SYNTAX - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 161
About This Presentation
Title:

SYNTAX

Description:

syntax lin 1310 – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:123
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 162
Provided by: EtaSchne
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: SYNTAX


1
SYNTAX
  • LIN 1310

2
  • Syntax is the component of grammar that deals
    with sentence structure.
  • Before we can talk about syntax, we need to
    discuss the nature of linguistic competence and
    the nature of grammar

3
The nature of grammar
  • Linguistic competence
  • Theoretically permits infinite production of
    novel utterances, including sentences.
  • How can our brains handle a system capable of
    infinite production?
  • Answer The elements of the system are not
    infinite. They are finite in number.

4
Grammar
  • A system that permits infinite creativity with a
    relatively limited number of elements, including
    speech sounds and the rules for combining them
    into words and sentences.
  • The rules for making sentences are called the
    syntax of the language.
  • What are these rules like and how are they stored
    as part of competence?

5
The Rules of Syntax
  • Does knowing the rules of syntax involve storing
    the syntactic structures of all possible
    sentences in our heads as a set of templates with
    slots to fill?
  • No, it does not.
  • Here are for reasons why not

6
Reason 1
  • 1. Rules of syntax allow sentences to be
    infinitely long. (The only limits are imposed by
    performance.)

7
Reason 2
  • 2. All possible sentences have not yet been
    uttered.

8
Reason 3
  • 3. Our storage capacity is finite.

9
Reason 4
  • 4. Sentence interpretation is structure
    dependent.
  • That is, it relies on more than a simple linear
    (one word after another) organization.
  • (This will be explained further.)

10
The Nature of Syntax
  • The following slides present a number of
    illustrations of the nature of syntax and facts
    about language that a linguistic theory of syntax
    must account for.

11
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 1
  • 1. How words in a sentence are assigned specific
    roles with respect to one another.
  • For example, in English active sentences
  • The first noun is the subject or doer of the
    action or agent.
  • The second noun is the object or receiver (or
    theme or patient) of the action.

12
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 1
  • Thus different interpretations for
  • The dog chased the cat.
  • subject verb object
  • The cat chased the dog.
  • subject verb object

13
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 1
  • Word order varies across languages.
  • In German, its subject, object, verb
  • Das Kind wird die Schwester lehren.
  • The child will the sister teach.
  • subject object verb

14
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 1
  • For some languages, like Welsh, the preferred
    order is verb, subject, object.
  • For example
  • darllenais i y llyfr
  • read I the book
  • verb subject object

15
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 1
  • Other languages may rely more on some form of
    morphological marking than on word ordering to
    assign roles to words in sentences.
  • However, these languages still have a most common
    or canonical word order.
  • The other permissible word orders tend to
    slightly alter the focus of the sentence.
  • See Japanese example on the next slide.

16
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 1
  • Canonical word order in Japanese
  • Yumiko scolded the child
  • Yumiko-ga sono kodomo-o sikat-ta
  • yumiko-NOM that child-ACC scold-past
  • subject object verb
  • Alternative order (focus on object)
  • Sono kodomo-o Yumiko-ga sikat-ta
  • that child-ACC yumiko-NOM scold-past
  • object subject verb

17
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 1
  • The most typical word order in a language is
    called its canonical word order.
  • 95 of the worlds languages have one of the
    following canonical word orders
  • SVO Canadians like hockey. (English)
  • SOV Canadians hockey like. (German)
  • VSO Like Canadians hockey. (Welsh)
  • VOS OVS OSV rare or nonexistent

18
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 2
  • Sentences with different surface structures and
    word orders can have the same interpretation of
    whos doing what to whom, even in a language with
    strict word order and no specific marking of
    subjects and objects.
  • Examine the following sentence pairs from
    English

19
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 2
  • scrambled order
  • The boy gave the toy to the girl.
  • direct indirect
  • object object
  • The boy gave the girl the toy.
  • indirect direct
  • object object

20
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 2
  • active
  • The student solved the problem.
  • (agent/subject) (theme/object)
  • passive
  • The problem was solved by the student.
  • (theme/object) (agent/subject)

21
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 2
  • declarative
  • Everyone is happy.
  • (verb)
  • yes/no question
  • Is everyone happy?
  • (verb)

22
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 2
  • declarative
  • You have been there.
  • auxiliary
  • yes/no question
  • Have you been there?
  • auxiliary

23
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 3
  • 3.Sentences contain discontinuous elements which
    are understood as part of the same structural
    constituent, as in
  • perfect (auxiliary have suffix -en)
  • I have eaten.
  • progressive (auxiliary be suffix -ing)
  • I am eating.

24
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 3
  • Wh question
  • What are they looking at?
  • Object
  • Declarative
  • They are looking at the dog.
  • object
  • NB Although what has moved to the front of the
    question sentence, it plays the same syntactic
    role as the dog does in the declarative. Both
    are part of the predicate.

25
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 4
  • 4. It is possible for sentences to be embedded
    inside other sentences.
  • The property of grammar which permits such
    embedding is called recursiveness.

26
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 4
  • Recursiveness also allows sentence constituents
    (parts) to occur inside other constituents. See
    the string of adjectives for the subject noun
    dog in the following example
  • The big gray shaggy friendly dog brought me the
    newspaper.

27
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 4
  • In the following examples, note how a sentence
    can grow as more strings of adjectives,
    prepositional phrases and embedded sentences are
    added
  • The dog was sleeping.
  • subject
  • The big dog was sleeping.
  • subject dog one adjective

28
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 4
  • The big ugly guard dog at the factory was
    sleeping.
  • Subject dog string of adjectives
    prepositional phrase

29
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 4
  • The big ugly guard dog at the factory which
    produces pillows that some people are allergic to
    was sleeping on the road that leads to the quarry
    where they found the hobo who had been strangled
    with a shoelace that was later shown to have been
    stolen from the woman on Main Street whose house
    was broken into last week.
  • subject dog string of adjectives
    prepositional phrases embedded sentences

30
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 5
  • 5. Sentences with the same apparent structural
    elements and word orders can have different
    meanings or interpretations.
  • John is easy to please
  • John is eager to please
  • Subject verb adjective infinitive
  • On the surface, the structures of sentences 1
    and 2 appear to be the same.

31
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 5
  • John is easy to please
  • John is eager to please
  • In sentence 1, John is unspoken/understood
    object of to please
    subject ? (somebody
  • In sentence 2, John is unspoken/understood,)
  • subject of to please
    object ? (somebody)

32
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 6
  • 6. A single sentence may have more than one
    meaning or interpretation.
  • This is called ambiguity.
  • Structural ambiguity
  • The two meanings hinge on different underlying
    relationships between the words in the sentence.
  • Example
  • Visiting professors can be interesting.

33
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 6
  • The structural ambiguity of the sentence rests
    on the ambiguous structure of the phrase visiting
    professors.
  • Visiting professors can be interesting.
  • modifier noun
  • Meaning Professors who have come here
    temporarily from other universities.
  • or
  • Visiting professors can be interesting.
  • gerund noun
  • Meaning For someone to visit professors.

34
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 6
  • Lexical ambiguity
  • Alternative interpretations of a sentence rest
    on different meanings of homophonous lexical
    items.
  • Example
  • She ate her cottage cheese with relish.
  • relish condiment or enthusiasm.
  • The two words sound identical, but have
    different meanings.

35
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 7
  • Not all elements of a sentence appear on the
    surface, yet listeners can correctly interpret
    the sentence.
  • For example
  • Joe likes pizza and Jack does too.
  • missing element likes pizza
  • Both like pizza, but likes pizza appears only
    once.

36
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 7
  • Pronouns sometimes stand in for missing
    elements
  • Mary bought some ice cream and __ ate it.
  • Mary bought some ice cream and she ate it.
  • If Mary and she stand for the same
    individual, they are co-referenced.
  • It is also possible that she is co-referenced
    with someone else.

37
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 8
  • 8.There are quite strict rules for forming
    sentences which speakers must adhere to.
  • Syntactically ill-formed sentences are
    ungrammatical.
  • Eats the food the child.
  • Mary refused to allow that the children go to
    the concert.
  • ?Mary refused to allow that type of language.

38
Facts syntactic theory must account for Fact 9
  • 9. Syntactic and semantic well-formedness are
    independent of one another.
  • The following sentences are syntactically
    well-formed but semantically anomalous or
    nonsensical
  • Colourless green ideas sleep furiously.
  • A verb crumpled the milk.

39
Transformational/Generative Approach
  • To account for these and other facts, most
    linguists adopt a transformational generative
    (TG) approach to describing the syntactic
    component of grammar.
  • A TG approach emphasizes the search for Universal
    Grammar (UG).

40
Transformational/Generative Approach
  • UG presupposes that languages operate under a
    shared set of categories, operations and
    principles.
  • Although languages obviously differ from one
    another, their are common principles governing
    the way they form sentences.
  • These properties of UG are called language
    universals.

41
Transformational/Generative Approach
  • By using the same descriptive approach for all
    languages, linguists hope to identify language
    universals and gain a more complete understanding
    of UG.
  • At the syntactic level, there are clearly two
    subcomponents found in all languages
  • 1. a lexicon
  • 2. a computational system

42
Transformational/Generative Approach
  • The lexicon or mental dictionary lists the words
    (and morphemes) in a language.
  • The lexicon also includes information about each
    entry regarding
  • -pronunciation
  • -meaning
  • -form (root, affix, bound, free, etc.)
  • -syntactic category information? (noun, verb,
    etc.)

43
Transformational/Generative Approach
  • The computational system includes the operations
    that allow words to be combined into syntactic
    structures.
  • The computational system has two major
    components
  • Merge
  • Move

44
Transformational/Generative Approach
  • Merge allows the creation of phrases (parts of
    sentences) and the combining of the these phrases
    into sentences.
  • Move allows certain elements to be transported to
    a new position within a sentence.
  • An example of a moved element is the word what
    moving out of object position into sentence
    initial position in
  • What are they looking at?

45
Transformational/Generative Approach
  • We will examine the elements of syntax as
    follows
  • 1. The lexicon and the notion of syntactic
    categories
  • 2. The creation of phrases (phrase structure)
  • 3. The merge operation to form sentences
  • 4. Types of phrasal clausal complements
  • 5. Move

46
Transformational/Generative Approach
  • Transformational Generative theory has undergone
    many reformulations over the past 50 years.
  • We are operating with a recent version.
  • Thus you may encounter terms elsewhere that are
    roughly equivalent to the elements of syntax that
    we are examining.
  • We will discuss these terms at a later date.

47
The Lexicon/Syntactic Categories
  • There is some controversy in current linguistic
    theory regarding whether the syntactic category
    (noun, verb, etc.) of a word is stored or
    syntactically determined. There are essentially
    two positions.
  • 1. This information is stored in the lexicon.
  • 2. The syntactic category is only determined when
    the word enters into a syntactic structure.
  • We will not resolve the issue in this class. We
    will assume position 1, although the reasoning
    behind position 2 may be discussed.

48
The Lexicon/Syntactic Categories
  • All languages group their words into syntactic
    categories.
  • We find remarkably similar syntactic categories
    across languages
  • Your textbook provides example of the commonly
    found categories in Table 5.1.1 on page 133.

49
(No Transcript)
50
The Lexicon/Syntactic Categories
  • The Lexical/Nonlexical distinction is roughly
    equivalent to the distinction we used in
    morphology
  • OpenClass/Content or Lexical Words
  • versus
  • ClosedClass/Function Words
  • or Non-lexical
  • The chart is reproduced in the next slide

51
Non-lexical/ functional
Lexical
Non-lexical/functional
(Closed Class)
52
The Lexicon/Syntactic Categories
  • The classification of words into syntactic
    categories can rely on
  • the type of meaning they express
  • what inflectional affixes they take
  • the types of structures they fit into
    (distribution)
  • Classification relies on a combination of these
    approaches.

53
Classifying Syntactic Categories
  • We will apply these 3 types of classification in
    the following slides.
  • This will hopefully allow you to gain a better
    understanding of Syntactic Categories (aka parts
    of speech)

54
Classifying Syntactic CategoriesMeaning
  • The meanings of Lexical (Open Class/Content)
    words are generally much easier to define that
    those of Non-lexical (Closed Class/Function)
    words.
  • Consider the words
  • the (Non-lexical/Closed Class - determiner)
    and
  • brush (Lexcial/Open Class - noun)

55
the versus brush
  • Its easier to explain the function of the, or
    where it occurs (before nouns), than what it
    actually means.
  • Its easier to describe the concept brush than
    the concept the.

56
Meanings of the Major Lexical Categories
  • Nouns name entities objects (book)
  • people (Mary)
  • Verbs designate actions (eat)
  • sensations (feel)
  • states (be, seem)
  • Adjectives designate properties and attributes
    of what nouns name (heavy book)
  • Adverbs Denote properties and attributes of
    what verbs designate (eat quickly)
  • Adverbs tell us how, why, where, when.

57
Meanings of the Major Lexical Categories
  • Meaning is not always a clear-cut way of deciding
    the part of speech of word.
  • Some lexical/content words are more difficult to
    define.
  • For example, the concepts truth or honesty do
    not name entities.

58
Meanings of the Major Lexical Categories
  • Items from two different categories can have
    similar meanings.
  • For example, the verb hate and the noun
    revulsion have very similar meanings
  • I hate carrots.
  • I feel revulsion toward carrots.

59
Meanings of the Major Lexical Categories
  • Some words, like brush, can be a noun or a
    verb.
  • I bought a new brush (noun) for the dog.
  • I brush (verb) the dog every day.

60
Inflections for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • So we still need to go beyond simple meaning to
    define a lexical (content) words syntactic
    category.
  • We can also look at what inflections a form
    takes, as inflections usually attach to a
    particular part of speech.
  • Note the references to parts of speech in the
    following list of English inflections.

61
The inflectional affixes of English
62
Inflections for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • Thus we can tell if ship is a noun or a verb
    by seeing whether it takes affixes that normally
    attach to a verb or a noun.
  • ship pl. s as in
  • I like to sail on ships.
  • ship poss. s as in
  • The ships hull is painted red.
  • So far, it looks as if ship is a noun.

63
Inflections for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • To be certain, lets see if adjectival inflections
    can be attached to ship.
  • Comparative er as in
  • That one is shipper than the first one.
  • Superlative est as in
  • That is shippest one of all.
  • So ship is not an adjective.

64
Inflections for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • What about verbal inflections?
  • Past tense ed as in
  • They shipped the package last week.
  • Progressive ing as in
  • They are shipping the package by air.
  • So, it looks as if ship is a verb as well as a
    noun.

65
Inflections for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • In fact, many words can belong to more than one
    lexical category.
  • For example
  • brush (noun, verb)
  • comb (noun, verb)
  • near (preposition, verb, adjective)
  • They got bored near the end.
  • They neared the finish line.
  • The are nearer to the end than us.

66
Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • We have seen that neither the meaning nor the
    affix test tells us reliably which lexical
    category a word belongs to.
  • Another more reliable way of determining lexical
    category is by looking at a words distribution.

67
Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • By distribution, we mean the type of elements
    that a word can co-occur with.
  • The most useful type of elements for
    distributional analysis are what functional
    categories a word can be used with, although we
    can also look at what lexical categories it can
    be used with.

68
Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • For example
  • nouns occur with determiners such as the
  • the dog
  • verbs occur with auxiliaries such as will
  • will go
  • adjectives occur with degree words such as
    very
  • very hot

69
Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • Verbs do not occur with determiners
  • the go
  • Nouns do not occur with auxiliaries such as
    will
  • will lamp
  • Neither nouns nor verbs occur with degree words
    such as very
  • very lamp very go

70
Homework
  • Study Guide Page 104
  • OA Page 170-171, Ex. 2
  • Think about how you could have used
    distribution, meaning and inflection tests in
    order to identify the syntactic categories (parts
    of speech) of the underlined words and words in
    the sentences of Ex. 2.
  • Refer to Tables 5.1, 5.2 and 5.3 in OA for
    help.

71
Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • Distributional analysis helps us to see the
    difference between verbs and auxiliaries.
  • A verb can co-occur with an auxiliary, but a verb
    cannot co-occur with another verb.
  • We may eat.
  • We have eaten.
  • We are eating.
  • We play eat.




72
Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • Sentences normally have only one verb.
  • So a sentence with more than one verb is really
    more than one sentence.
  • Such as two conjoined sentences as in
  • Theyre moving and grooving.
  • From Theyre moving. Theyre grooving.

73
Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • Or, two verbs might be a clue that there is a
    main sentence containing an embedded or
    subordinate sentence or infinitival complement as
    in
  • We are going to play.
  • go is the main verb and play is the verb in
    the infinitival complement to play.

74
Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • Further distributional analysis can show us that
    there are actually three types of auxiliary in
    English
  • Modals (will, would, shall, should, can, could,
    must, may, might)
  • Perfect (have en)
  • Progressive (be-ing)

75
Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • Lets look at modals first.
  • Modals can co-occur with verbs
  • He will eat. He may eat.
  • Modals cannot co-occur with modals
  • He may will eat.

76
Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • You cannot replace a verb with a modal as in
  • We are maying.
  • Nor can you replace a modal with a verb as in
  • We wish be eating.

77
Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • Modals can co-occur with perfect have.
  • He may have eaten.
  • Modals can co-occur with progressive be.
  • He may be eating.
  • Notice that when modals co-occur with any of
    these elements, the modal always comes first.

78
Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • Lets try a substitution test to determine whether
    the following are modals or verbs
  • Try substituting each of the following words for
    will in the following sentence
  • can, play, could, shall, see
  • He will eat.
  • Which ones are OK?

79
Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • Now try substituting the same words for the verb
    eat in
  • He will eat.
  • can, play, could, shall, see
  • Which ones are OK?
  • So our substitution test tells us that
  • can, could shall are modals
  • play and see are verbs

80
Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • Modals always occur before perfect and
    progressive (when they are actually present)
    and before a verb.
  • Verbs occur after all possible elements in the
    auxiliary.

81
Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • How do we know that perfect have -en is not a
    modal?
  • Try the substitution test on
  • He will have eaten.
  • Can you substitute a modal for have?
  • He will may eaten?
  • Can you reverse the order of will and have?
  • He have will eaten?

82
Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • How do we know that progressive be -ing is not
    a modal?
  • Try the substitution test on
  • He will have eaten.
  • He be having eaten.

83
Distribution for Determining Major Lexical
Categories
  • How do we know that progressive be ing and
    perfect have en are separate categories?
  • Try reversing them in
  • He may have been eating.
  • He may be having eaten.

84
Still not convinced about modals?
  • Lets apply the meaning test.
  • Modals express the speakers attitude or
    intentions with respect to the sentence. Modals
    can express possibility, probability, necessity,
    intention, etc., as in
  • I will go.
  • I may go.
  • I must go.

85
Still not convinced about modals?
  • Lets apply the inflection test.
  • Unlike verbs, modals do not take the present
    tense s inflection in the 3rd person singular
    verbs.
  • He eats at nine.
  • He wills eat at nine.
  • He mays eat at nine.

86
Homework
  • We can apply the distribution and meaning tests
    to identify perfect and progressive as distinct
    functional categories.
  • The meanings of sentences with perfect and
    progressive are affected in interesting ways.
  • Think about the meanings of the sentences with
    perfect and progressive on the following slide.
  • Be prepared to discuss them in your DGD.

87
Perfect and Progressive
  • John has studied karate, but he is more
    interested in kickboxing.
  • John had studied karate for ten years before he
    got his black belt.
  • I lived in Florida for ten years. Then I moved
    to Texas.
  • I have lived in Florida for ten years, but I
    still miss Canada.
  • I had lived in Florida for ten years before I
    saw my first alligator.
  • I cant talk while I am driving.
  • I didnt use my cell while I was driving.
  • I am driving to Montreal tomorrow.
  • Think about the concept of tense in English and
    what it expresses
  • I am exercising right now. (present progressive
    - ongoing)
  • I exercise every day. (simple present -
    habitual)
  • I exercised every day last week. (simple past -
    completed)
  • I was exercising when you called me last week.
    (past progressive - ongoing in past)

88
Perfect
  • Perfect have-en expresses the duration of an
    event as in I have lived in Florida for ten
    years. In the past tense it can also express the
    duration of an event with respect to a certain
    point in the past, as in I had lived in Florida
    for ten years when they discovered I was an
    illegal alien. Perfect have-en can also be used
    to refer to an event that took place over some
    indefinite period in the past, even when the
    sentence is actually inflected for the present,
    as in I have studied karate.
  • Perfect have-en is rarely used to indicate a
    completed act. Instead English uses the simple
    past tense as in I lived in Florida for ten
    years. Compare that to the sense of duration in
    I have lived in Florida for 10 years. Note also
    that the simple present in English cannot express
    duration as in I know John for ten years.
    However, the simple present in English does
    express habitual actions as in I eat breakfast
    at seven.

89
Progressive
  • Progressive be-ing refers to events in progress
    in either the present or the past. In the
    present, the event is understood as happening
    while the sentence is being uttered. In the
    past, the event is understood as happening while
    another event in the discourse was happening.
    Progressive can also communicate anticipation of
    an event which will happen as in I am going
    shopping later. There is a narrative discourse
    style in English that uses the present
    progressive and the simple present to relate
    events that occurred in the story, as in Im
    walking down the street last night and this guy
    jumps out from behind a parked car and starts
    yelling at me (etc.)

90
A word about auxiliaries
  • English has four different functional categories
    that can be described as auxiliaries
  • tense, modal, perfect, progressive
  • If modal, perfect and progressive are all in a
    sentence, they must occur in that specific order
    for the sentence to be grammatical.
  • We will discuss tense placement later.

91
From Word Categories to Phrases
  • Sentences are made up of sub-parts called
    phrases.
  • Every language has a set of rules for forming
    these phrases.
  • They are called Phrase Structure Rules
  • The Phrase Structure Rules are a set of rules
    that allow us to map out the structures of
    phrases in a language.
  • We diagram these structures as inverted trees.

92
From Word Categories to Phrases
  • The inverted trees reflect the hierarchical
    arrangement of phrases.
  • Sentences consist of a series of phrases also
    joined together in a hierarchical manner.

93
Merge X (X-bar)
  • Merge is the part of the syntax in which of
    words are fitted together into phrases and in
    which phrases are joined together to form
    sentences.
  • Merge combines words into phrases using the X
    schema that we will be discussing shortly.
  • The Phrase Structure Rules determining these
    phrases used to be fairly elaborate, but in the
    current formulation of the theory have been
    essentially streamlined to the X schema.

94
Merge Subcategorization
  • Phrases are also the product of an interaction
    between the X schema and the subcategorization
    properties of words.
  • Subcategorization refers to the type of
    complement structures that certain words must or
    can appear with.
  • to hate (verb) requires a direct object
  • I hate. I hate carrots.

95
I hate carrots.

96
Merge D-structure
  • Merge results in what used to be called the Deep
    (D) Structure of a sentence
  • The D-structure of a sentence very closely
    resembles the canonical word order in the
    language.
  • D structure is not, however, the final form of
    the sentence.

97
Move
  • Not all sentences have the canonical word order
    in the language.
  • Thus another component of syntax, called Move,
    moves elements to where they belong in the
    surface structure or S-structure of the sentence.
  • Compare
  • John is eating an apple. (canonical)
  • Is John eating an apple? (non-canonical)

98
Why D- and Surface Structure?
  • We just saw that not all sentences follow
    canonical word order.
  • But why not allow Merge to create these
    structures in the first place?
  • Why do we need Move?
  • I will provide some reasons in the next few
    slides and point out others as we explore the
    syntax more thoroughly.

99
Why D- and Surface Structure?
  • By adhering to strict canonical word order,
    D-structure gives us important information about
    the semantic roles of the elements in a
    sentences, especially the main nouns with respect
    to the verb.
  • Thus, for English, the first Noun Phrase
    constituent in the tree represents the subject of
    the sentence.
  • The subject of the sentence has a special
    relationship to the verb, as it is the doer of
    the action or agent.

100
Why D- and Surface Structure?
  • The first Noun Phrase immediately following a
    transitive verb is its direct object or theme.
  • In semantics, these relationships of nouns with
    respect to verbs and their subjects and objects
    are called thematic roles.
  • The students read the book.
  • agent theme

101
Why D- and Surface Structure?
  • The thematic roles of the constituents of the
    sentence would not be so apparent at D-structure
    if non-canonically ordered trees were permitted.
  • The move component of the syntax can operate on
    D-structure trees to create these non-canonical
    orders as required.

102
Why D- and Surface Structure?
  • At D-structure it is clear when two sentences
    have the same thematic relationships between
    their words.
  • Recall the sentences we looked at a few classes
    ago

103
Why D- and Surface Structure?
  • declarative
  • Everyone is happy.
  • (verb)
  • yes/no question
  • Is everyone happy?
  • (verb)

104
Why D- and Surface Structure?
  • Wh question
  • What are they looking at?
  • object
  • theme
  • Declarative
  • They are looking at what.
  • object
  • theme

105
Why D- and Surface Structure?
  • scrambled order
  • The boy gave the toy to the girl.
  • direct indirect object
  • theme goal
  • The boy gave the girl the toy.
  • indirect direct
  • goal theme

106
Why D- and Surface Structure?
  • By having the alternative word orders created by
    Move, there is no need for extra phrase structure
    rules that would create essentially the same
    structure in two different places in the tree.
  • So we dont need a rule in Merge permitting a
    verb to occur at the beginning of a tree as well
    as after the subject
  • Is everyone happy? Everyone is happy.
  • We also dont risk confusion about whether the
    role of the nouns everyone and what in the
    question sentences.

107
A schematic of syntax
  • Merge
  • Deep Structure
  • Move
  • Syntactic Surface Structure

108
Is that all there is to the grammar?
  • In the previous slide we saw that Merge creates
    D-structure which is acted upon by Move which in
    turn creates Syntactic Surface Structure.
  • Is that it for the grammar? No.
  • The syntactic surface structure does not
    represent the final spoken form of the utterance.
  • The rules of phonology and semantic
    interpretation (logical form) must apply before
    the sentence is fully derived.

109
From theory to practice
  • Merge is the product of the X schema.
  • This basically sets out the structure of a
    typical, generic phrase as
  • XP
  • (Specifier) X
  • X (complement)

110
From theory to practice
  • The abbreviations in the generic X phrase
    structure tree Xpart of speech of the head of
    the phrase
  • Pphrase (e.g. NP noun phrase)
  • Specifierphrase boundary marker, makes
    meaning of head more precise
  • Complementphrases which provide information
    about the meaning of the head. The type of
    complement a head can take is part of the
    information stored with it in the mental
    lexicon.
  • NB Parentheses ( ) mean that an element is
    optional in some phrases and with some heads.

111
Types of X - Heads
  • Heads can be Nouns, Verbs, Adjectives,
    Prepositions
  • At the sentence level, heads are called
  • I Inflection C Complementizer
  • We will discuss sentence heads later.

112
Specifiers (abbreviations in bold)
  • The following can act as Specifiers
  • Determiners (for nouns N)
  • e.g. the, a, this, these, those, no,
  • Adverbs (for verbs V)
  • e.g. never, perhaps, often, always
  • Degree word (for Adjectives A, Prepositions P)
  • e.g. very, quite, more, almost

113
Complements
  • Phrases that add information about entities and
    locations implied by the head and for which the
    head is subcategorized.
  • In the following example, the NP carrots is the
    complement of the verb hate.
  • to hate requires a direct object complement
  • I hate. I hate carrots.

114
Other Complement Examples
  • prepositional phrases as in
  • I often eat at that restaurant.
  • I never approved of that purchase.
  • I was certain of his loyalty.
  • The destruction of the city angered me.
  • 1 and 2 show verb complements,
  • 3 shows an adjective complement.
  • 4 shows a noun complement.

115
Practice drawing phrase structures
  • Keep in mind that all phrases follow the x
    schema.
  • XP
  • (Specifier) X
  • X (complement)

116
(No Transcript)
117
(No Transcript)
118
He often reads tons of books

119
He often reads tons of books

120
He often reads tons of books
121
How to recognize a phrase
  • Look for typical heads, such as
  • noun, verb, adjective, preposition
  • Work from right to left, since English branches
    right and your lowest phrases in the tree will
    likely be found to the right.
  • Consider possible structures of phrase and see
    how X applies

122
Complements
  • N B See OA Charts 5.5 (p143), 5.6, 5.7, 5.8
    (p144) for types of complements

123
Verb Complements
124
Noun, adjective, preposition complements
125
Phrase Tests
  • You can also try the phrase tests of
  • substitution
  • movement
  • coordination

126
Substitution Test
  • nouns can be replaced by they it
  • The boys bought a firecracker.
  • verbs can be replaced by do so
  • The children can play.
  • prepositional phrase can be replaced by there
  • He went to the bank.

127
Movement Test
  • prepositional phrases can often be moved without
    compromising the grammaticality of the sentence
  • The children sang in the chapel.
  • In the chapel the children sang.

128
Coordination Test
  • a phrase can be conjoined with another phrase of
    the same type using and
  • They mowed the lawn.
  • They mowed the lawn and raked the leaves.

129
Sally saw the car on the hill.
  • - hill is a noun, so it must be part of an NP
  • the is determiner, so it must be the specifier
    of the NP the hill

130
Sally saw the car on the hill.
131
Sally saw the car on the hill.
  • on is a preposition, which is the head of a PP
  • PPs have NPs as their complement, so the hill
    is the complement of the PP with of.

132
Sally saw the car on the hill.
133
Sally saw the car on the hill.
  • car is a noun, so it must be the head of an NP
  • the is a determiner, so it must be the
    specifier of the NP with car
  • on the hill is a PP which acts as a complement
    to car

134
Sally saw the car on the hill.
135
Sally saw the car on the hill.
  • -saw is a verb, which acts as the head of a VP
  • -saw is subcategorized for an NP complement
  • -the NP complement of saw is the car

136
Sally saw the car on the hill.
137
Sally saw the car on the hill.
  • Sally is a noun, which is the head of an NP

138
Sally saw the car on the hill.
139
But how does it all fit together?
  • Using X, the theory treats the subject NP as the
    specifier of a new phrase called IP
  • The theory treats VP of the sentence as the
    complement of this phrase.
  • The head of the phrase is the tense inflection of
    the sentence I.
  • Remember that English has only two tenses
  • past (pst) present (-pst)

140
Sally saw the car on the hill.
141
Try The dog bit the cat.
142
The dog bit the cat.
143
IP
  • IP allows us to deal with the tense and modal
    parts of the auxiliary of the sentence.
  • The remaining parts of the auxiliary (perfect
    have and progressive be)
  • are handled a little differently, as we will see.

144
Modals in IP
  • The theory treats models has the lexical part of
    I of the IP.
  • This is because models have inherent tense.
  • We never add a tense inflection to make them past
    or present.
  • He will eat. (-pst, NB no s in 3rd pers.
    sg.)
  • He would eat if he had money (-pst)
  • He would eat whenever anyone fed him. (pst)

145
Modals in IP
  • You judge the tense of the modal based on the
    rest of the sentence or discourse.
  • If there is a modal, the verb is not marked for
    tense.
  • The dog will bite the cat.
  • The dog bites the cat.

146
Modals in IP
  • Note that if there is no Modal, the tense in the
    PS tree matches the inflection on the verb.
  • This may seem bizarre, but syntactic theory has
    had a long-standing conflict of over whether and
    how to handle inflectional morphology.
  • Compare

147
(No Transcript)
148
(No Transcript)
149
Modals in IP
  • Any modal could take the place of will in The
    dog will bite the cat.
  • The dog will bite the cat.
  • The dog would bite the cat.
  • The dog may bite the cat.
  • The dog can bite the cat.
  • The dog could bite the cat.
  • The dog should bite the cat.
  • The dog must bite the cat.
  • The dog might bite the cat.
  • The dog shall bite the cat.

150
Perfect and Progressive
  • If the sentence contains the auxiliaries perfect
    or progressive or both, these are treated as
    verbs.
  • This will give us successive VPs as follows

151
1
2
152
Modal, Perfect and Progressive
  • In The dog has bitten the cat, note that tense
    is on perfect have, not on the verb.
  • Tense with perfect have is relative to the
    other events in the discourse.
  • Compare a sentence with modal and perfect.
  • The dog will have bitten the cat.
  • Here tense is expressed through modal will,
    rather than on have.

153
(No Transcript)
154
(No Transcript)
155
(No Transcript)
156
Modal-Perfect-Progressive
  • In older versions of the theory, the affixes
    associated with perfect have (-en) and
    progressive be (-ing) were shown together with
    them as unattached inflections in the PS.
  • Movement took place to attach them where they
    belonged.
  • Nowadays, syntacticians do not want morphological
    operations within syntax.
  • Thus they are glossed over in the PS trees.

157
Whole Sentences as Complements
  • What if our complement is a whole sentence
    embedded in the bigger sentence.
  • We use the term clause for sentence.
  • Hence embedded clause.
  • A clause must have a verb.

158
Embedded Clauses
  • I know that Mary has left.
  • Compare it to
  • I know the answer.
  • The structures are both complements of the
    verb, but one is a clause (aka sentence).
  • The embedded clause acts like an NP.

159
Embedded Clauses
  • Embedded clauses that replace NPs are marked by
    the words that or whether or if.
  • These are called complementizers.
  • They form the head of a CP (complementizer
    phrase) that goes into the complement position of
    the VP.

160
(No Transcript)
161
(No Transcript)
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com