Affixation Conventionalization Hypothesis

1 / 20
About This Presentation
Title:

Affixation Conventionalization Hypothesis

Description:

... syllable nucleus could be lengthened. Vowel lengthening? ... No vowel lengthening upon possession. Other potential counterexamples. Synharmony Not Default ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:63
Avg rating:3.0/5.0

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Affixation Conventionalization Hypothesis


1
Affixation Conventionalization Hypothesis
  • An Explanation of Conventional Spellings in Mayan
    Writing
  • David F. Mora-Marín
  • davidmm_at_unc.edu

2
The Problem Fictitious Vowels
  • Basic Mayan root shape CVC
  • Basic suffix shapes -VC, -CVC
  • Script
  • CVC(VC) logograms
  • CV syllabograms (very few CVC syllabograms)
  • Spelling of CVC roots, CVC-VC or CVC-CVC stems
    typically results in a fictitious vowel
  • Some such spellings show a synharmonic
    fictitious vowel C1V1C2-C2(V1) or C1V1-C2 (V1)
  • E.g. ka-ka for kahk fire
  • Generally considered default
  • Some show a disharmonic fictitious vowel
    C1V1C2-C2 (V2) or C1V1-C2 (V2)
  • E.g. ba-ki for ba(a)k bone
  • Problematized

3
Typology of Proposals
  • Knorozov 1967
  • Justeson 1978
  • Bricker 1989
  • Justeson 1989
  • Hopkins n.d., 2004
  • Houston et al. 1998, 2004
  • Lacadena and Wichmann 2004
  • Justeson 2000
  • Mora-Marín 2001, 2002, 2004
  • Kaufman 2003
  • Boot 2003, 2004
  • Anderson 2004
  • A few others
  • Orthographically-oriented
  • Silent vowels
  • Neutral vowels
  • Underspellings
  • Morphological conditioning
  • Affixation conventionalization (typical
    suffixing)
  • Phonetically or phonologically-oriented
  • Echo vowels (phonetic)
  • Assimilation to preceding consonant (labial for
    Ca vs. apical for Ci)
  • Diacritical of complexity of preceding vowel
    nucleus
  • Most approaches regard synharmony as default

4
Disharmony Hypothesis (DH)
  • Houston et al. 1998, 2004
  • Claim 1 Synharmonic spellings are default
  • Claim 2 Disharmonic spellings represent
  • CVC (vowel length)
  • CVhC (post-V h)
  • CVC (post-V )
  • Claim 3 No exceptions
  • Claim 4 Disharmonic spellings show vowel length
    was preserved in language of texts
  • Claim 5 Late Classic increase of synharmonic
    spellings supports loss of phonemic V vs. V
    distinction
  • (Claim 6 Suffixes otherwise underrepresented due
    to disharmony principle are represented
    logographically by morphosyllables)

5
Problems with DH
  • First, there are counterexamples
  • Second, it is acontextual
  • Third, synharmonic spellings are not default
  • Phonological conditioning
  • Morphological conditioning
  • Underrepresentation
  • Fourth, disharmonic spellings can be accounted
    for by
  • Morphological conditioning
  • Underrepresentation
  • Fifth, there is no need for morphosyllables if
    simple spelling rules are assumed
  • Sixth, it uses spellings of terms with uncertain
    phonological shapes or etymologies

6
How to Test Models
  • Problems with DH
  • Could other approaches work?
  • Test of underrepresentation approach
  • Study variant spellings of same root across
    identical morphosyntactic contexts
  • Test of typical suffixing approach
  • Study identical spellings of same root across
    different morphosyntactic contexts
  • Study typical suffixes same roots take in modern
    descendant languages
  • Do not assume most synharmonic spellings are
    default
  • Check for additional phonological conditions
    leading to synharmonic or disharmonic spellings
  • Mora-Marín 2001, 2002/2003
  • Kaufman 2003

7
Counterexamples
  • yi-cha-ki for y-ihchak(-il) 3sERG-claw(-POSS)
    its claw
  • Proto-Mayan iSkaq
  • H et al. 1998, 2004 discount yi-cha-ki example
    they argue its reconstruction is uncertain
  • Since it is possessed, one could argue that its
    second syllable nucleus could be lengthened
  • Vowel lengthening?
  • POP y-iskaq
  • MAM t-xkyaq
  • PQM ERG-ixkaq
  • KCH r-ixkaq
  • QAN y-isqaq chej casco de caballo
  • No vowel lengthening upon possession
  • Other potential counterexamples

8
Synharmony Not Default
  • Justeson 1989 Obligatory synharmony
  • C1VC1 roots ka-k(a) for kahk fire,
    ta-t(a) for tät thick
  • CV roots mo-(o) for mo macaw, TE(-e) for
    te tree
  • Exceptions are contextually accounted for
  • Bricker 2002, Mora-Marín 2002, 2003
  • C1VC1 sequences -le-l(e) for -lel
    abstractive, tzu-nu-n(u) for tzu()nu()n
    hummingbird
  • CV sequences KABA(-a) for ka()ba()
    name

9
Phonological Conditioning I
  • Also other cases of V-deletion upon addition of
    suffixes (and an additional syllable or two)
  • chu-ka-j(a) chu-k(u)-ji-ya, suggests chuk-aj
    vs. chuk-j-iy(-a)
  • Mora-Marín 2002, 2003
  • First Context VCCV sequences
  • a-k(a)-ta for ahkt-a(j)-Ø (/ahkot-aj/)
    dance-IVZR-3sABS s/he dance(s/d)
  • yo-k(o)-bi-l(i) for y-ok-b-il
    (/y-ok-ib-il/) 3sERG-enter-INSTR-POSS his
    entrance/step (yo-ki-bi also attested, showing
    stem without -il)
  • Includes ALL positionals, which typically take
    -CVC suffixes

10
Phonological Conditioning II
  • Mora-Marín 2002, 2003
  • Second context VC-V sequences
  • yi-l(i)-a-ji for y-il-ä 3sERG-see-APPL s/he
    sees/saw it
  • More common spelling yi-la-ji (no morpheme
    boundary recognized)
  • Also u-PAT(-ta)-(t)i-ji, for u-pat-ij (cf.
    MacLeod 2004)
  • Morphographemic spellings

11
Morphological Conditioning
  • KIN-ni for kin day, sun
  • (u-)KIN-ni-li/le for u-kin-il
    3sPOSS/ERG-day/sun-POSS his/her special day
  • KIN-ni-chi for ltkinichgt Sun God
  • WAY-ya
  • WAY-ya-la for way-al alter ego
  • WAY-ya-si for way-as alter ego
  • Multiple reinforcement

12
Consonant Deletion
  • Mathews and Justeson 1984, Bricker 1989, Justeson
    1989, Zender 1999
  • ka-se-wa ka-se for ltcaseugt fifth month
    (Bricker 198945)
  • AJAW-le-l(e) AJAW-le for -lel abstractive
    (Bricker 198945)
  • u-to-ma u-to for uht-om be.finished-POT/FUT
    will be finished
  • C-deletion must be considered before regarding a
    given spelling as conventionalized
  • It can lead to seemingly synharmonic or
    disharmonic spellings of roots

13
Disharmonic Spellings Morphological Conditioning
I
  • CHAK-ki cha-ki
  • CHAK-ki cha-ki for chahk lightning, thunder
  • u-CHAK-ki-li u-cha-ki-li for u-chahk-il
    his/her/its lightning/thunder(?)

14
Morphological Conditioning II
  • a-na-bi
  • a-na-bi for ajnahb(-il) he of the lake
  • ya-na-bi-li for y-ajnahb-il his/her
    lake-person
  • Naj Tunich mural Aj Nahb(-il) saw it He
    is the Aj Nahb-il of Mr. So-and-So

15
C-deletion Test Variation in Equivalent Contexts
16
Affixation Conventionalization Hypothesis (ACH)
  • C1V1C2(-C2V2) or C1V1-C2 (V2) in phonetic
    complementation or phonetic spellings of
    root/word-closing segments the second vowel is
    likely to correspond to the vowel of the most
    common suffix or suffixes that a root may exhibit
    in the texts
  • Requires alternative spellings of same root
    across different morphosyntactic contexts (unique
    spellings are useless)
  • Can be tested against purely linguistic sources
    (e.g. Kaufman 2003a, 2003b), which can take on
    spellings attested across contexts, as well as
    unique spellings

17
Kaufman 2003a, 2003b
  • Kaufman uses primarily linguistic sources, but
    also epigraphic sources
  • Some of his data fills in epigraphic gaps in ACH
    approach
  • u-ne-na attested as s-nehn-al in Tzotzil
  • Some of the gaps in his data are filled in by
    epigraphic data of ACH approach
  • CHAK mu-ti-l(a) for red bird
  • AJ-to-ka-l(a) for ajtok-al

18
Reanalysis of H. et al.s Data Set
  • Disharmonic spellings
  • 15/35 spellings accounted for by ACH alone
  • 15/35 accounted for by linguistic data (including
    12 not accounted for by ACH)
  • 27/34 disharmonic spellings accounted for
  • Of remaining 8 spellings 5 are unique or
    invariant, 1 is of questionable etymology, and 2
    are CV-CV spellings of word-final -VC suffixes,
    which are overwhelmingly spelled with
    word-closing Ca signs if discounted, then the
    testable items are all accounted for
  • Synharmonic spellings
  • 14/39 spellings accounted for by ACH
  • 12/39 spellings accounted for by linguistic data
    alone (including 5 not accounted for by ACH)
  • 12/39 spellings accounted for by phonological
    conditioning
  • Total of 29/39 synharmonic spellings accounted
    for
  • Some spellings are unique or decontextualized

19
Conclusions
  • ACH has internal control No isolated or unique
    examples admitted
  • ACH depends on system-internal evidence only
    (actual attested spellings)
  • Underspellings are a fact, and theyre common
    (and attested in colonial Yukatek documents
    written by scribes who applied hieroglyphic
    conventions to the alphabetic medium)
  • Other phonetic, phonological accounts are
    unnecessary
  • (Does not rely on morphosyllables, only on
    simple, well-attested spelling rules)
  • Underspellings
  • Disharmonic
  • Synharmonic
  • Affixation Conventionalization
  • Disharmonic
  • Synharmonic
  • Phonological Contexts (C1VC1, CV, VCCV,
    VC-V)
  • Synharmonic Vowel Insertion
  • (Polymorphemic Logograms and phonetic complements
    (not MH))

20
Conclusions
  • DH does not recognize prevalence of
    underrepresentation
  • Does not recognize phonological conditions for
    synharmony
  • It is acontextual
  • Does not take affixation into account
  • Does not take syntax and pragmatics into account
  • Its decontextualization of spellings precludes
    testing of other approaches
  • Uses spellings of uncertain phonological shapes
    or etymologies
  • (Relies on hypothetical existence of
    morphosyllables)
  • There are counterexamples
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)