Title: 48x36 Poster Template
1A Production Study on Phonologization of
/u/-fronting in Alveolar Context KATAOKA, Reiko
(kataoka_at_berkeley.edu)Department of Linguistics,
University of California at Berkeley, 1203
Dwinelle Hall, Berkeley, California 94720-2650
Acoustic Analysis
Results
Introduction
Results (cont.)
- A. Realization of /u/ in /DuD/, /bud/, and
/hud/ (and Reference vowels) - Phonetic realization of /u/ are similar in /bud/
and /hud/. - Realization of /u/ in /DuD/ are distinctly
fronted (blue oval) (Figure 4). - Figure 4. Scatter plots for NF1 and NF2 for
medium speech rate tokens. For Test words, each
data point shows mean of five test words (dude,
zoos, noon, toot, Seus). (n31)
- A. F1 and F2 measurements from Reference vowels
- Figure 1. A sample waveform and spectrogram of
Thats a whod again, highlighting the vowel
u. - For all reference vowels, F1 and F2 were measured
at the temporal midpoint (red line) of the voiced
portion of the vowel (Figure 1). - 2) Then median F1 and F2 values were calculated
for the six tokens for each vowel category (i,
?, ?, æ, ?, ?, ?, u) for each
speaker. - F1 and F2 measurements from Test and Control
vowels
- NF2 as a function of segment duration
- NF2 of /u/ in /bud/ remain the same across
different segment duration. - NF2 of /u/ in /hud/ and /DuD/ increases
(decreases in decimal scale in Hz) as segment
duration increases. - NF2 of /u/ in /DuD/ have markedly different
distribution, and the regression line for this
data group does not cross the regression line for
/bud/ data group. - Regression lines for the /hvd/ and /DuD/ data are
nearly parallel (Figure 7).
Recently many researchers model sound change as
the result of phonologization (Hyman 1976), a
process whereby coarticulatory perturbation of a
speech sound becomes intended properties of the
sound. Although phonologization is a useful
concept in that it emphasizes the cognitive role
in sound change, that a contexually predictable
feature becomes dissociate from its contexts
(Ohala, 1981), diagnostic criteria for
determining whether phonologization has or has
not occurred when the context itself is still
present have yet to be proposed. This paper
demonstrates how production data of various
speech rates may be used to differentiate
phonologized speech variations from mechanically
caused variations. The case in point is
/u/-fronting in alveolar contexts in American
English, e.g., dude (/dud/) being pronounced as
d?d. On the one hand, the alveolar environment
would be expected to raise the 2nd formant of the
vowel, thus leading to the vowel being fronted
but is the extent of this fronting purely
phonetic or could it have become phonological?
Context /hid/ /hud/ /h?d/ /h?d/ /h?d/ /h?d/
/hæd/ /bud/ /h?t/ /DuD/ Type Control
/bud/ Ref /hVd/ Test /DuD/
Background Previous Studies on Coarticulatory
Variations
- Lindblom (1963) Vowel reduction in Swedish CVC
sequences - Found Reduced undershoot as segment duration
increases - Undershoot is due to automatic coarticulation
(i.e., phonetic). - Invariant vowel target articulation
- Solé (1992) Vowel nasalization in American
English and Spanish - Found Constant duration for nasalization in
Spanish vs. variable duration as a function of
segmental duration in English - In Spanish nasalization is purely phonetic
- In American English nasalization is phonologized
- Phonological features resist durational
manipulation. - Phonetic coarticulation disappears if there is
enough time for the articulator to reach its
target position.
Discussion
- The results from the production data obtained
from 31 speakers indicate that the high back
vowels had considerably higher F2 values
following alveolar consonants than bilabial or no
consonants (the phonetic realization of /h/ is a
voiceless copy of the following vowel). The
difference does not go away when segment duration
increases (up to 300 ms). This suggests that
speakers have distinct production goals for /u/s
in alveolar contexts that is, /u/-fronting in
this context has been phonologized in American
English. - The near parallel regression lines for the /DuD/
and /hud/ data further reinforce the idea of
separate articulatory goals for vowels in
different contexts. - For /u/ in /bud/, NF2 values remains the same
across speech rates. This seems to reflect the
fact that the onset /b/ and the vowel /u/ share
the same articulation and thus the lowest F2 is
not affected by the degree of overlapping of
adjacent articulations. - The results of this study seem to suggest that
language users have many more distinctive sound
categories than traditionally assumed. Further,
coarticulation may be better understood as
categorical phenomena than gradient phenomena.
Even if it starts out as gradient effect, by
using the same articulatory sequence again and
again, language learners might inevitably develop
stable categorical sequencial articulatory goals.
Mean duration (pooled) Fast 113.7 ms
Medium 151.6 ms Slow 215.7 ms
Materials and Methods
Following Lindblom (1963) and Solé (1992), the
question of whether /u/-fronting is phonetic or
phonological was investigated by production
experiment, where speakers were asked to produce
various words and non-words of a /C1VC2/ form in
carrier sentences, where the place of C2 was
fixed to alveolar and the place of C1 varied
among alveolar (e.g. /dud/), bilabial (e.g.,
/bud/) and no place (i.e., /hud/), in slow,
medium, and fast speech rate. Data collection
UC Berkeley, Phonology Lab Participants
Native speakers of American English 31
talkers (5 M, 10F 19-45 yrs old) Carrier
Sentence Thats a _____ again. Production
List (each word was repeated 6 times) Reference
hvd (medium rate) Test dvd (fast, slow,
medium rate) hed i dude dud hid ? zo
os zuz head ? noon nun had æ too
t tut hot ? Seus sus
(53690 tokens) HUD ? hood ? Control
cvc (fast, slow, medium rate) whod u
(8648 tokens) booed bud
whod hud (23636 tokens)
Total 174 tokens/speaker elicited
(ln)
vowel i u ? ?
? ? æ ?
ln(F1) 5.89 ln(F2) 8.02 NF1 0.51 NF2
-0.42
m_ln(F1) 6.4 m_ln(F2) 7.6
Bibliography
(ln)
Hyman, L. (1976). Phonologization, In
Linguistic studies presented to Joseph H.
Greenberg, edited by A. Juilland (Saratoga), pp.
407-418. Lindblom, B., and Studdert-Kennedy, M.
(1967). On the role of formant transitions in
vowel recognition, J. Acoust Soc. Am. 42,
830-843. Nearey, T. (1978). Phonetic Feature
Systems for Vowels. Indiana. Indiana University
Linguistics Club. Ohala, J. J. (1981). The
listener as a source of sound change, in Papers
from Parasession on Language and Behavior,
edited by C. S. Masek, R. A. Hendrick, and M. F.
Miller (Chicago Linguistic Society, Chicago), pp.
178-203. Solé, M. J. (1992). Phonetic and
phonological processes the case
of nasalization, Language and Speech 35, 2943.