Title: Constructing Animals: scientific, cultural and philosophical perspectives
1Constructing Animals scientific, cultural and
philosophical perspectives
- How do we think about animals?
- Attitudes and beliefs are shaped by cultural and
historical perspective - We need to dig deep to uncover philosophical
suppositions that structure scientific theory,
lay beliefs and popular understanding - Within society, animals inhabit various, often
contradictory positions and roles - The ways in which we think about animals, and the
meanings we place on them, are heavily structured
by the sorts of relationships we have with them.
2How do we categorise animals?
- Animals can be.meat, pets, companions, workers,
machines, guinea pigs, pests, little humans,
archetypes, brands. - Categories are shaped by culture, time, ethics,
philosophy, need, values - Our categories are not value-free e.g. wild vs
tame, good vs bad animals - Our categories are not fixed but are fuzzy (see
Lakoff Johnson, 1980). We sometimes refer to
humans as animals and animals as human-like. Our
categorisation is often contradictory
3Self and other
- Distinguishing self from other is one of the
first distinctions children must learn to make
(Berman, 1990). - Animals provide an important means to contrast
self with different others (or not so different
others) (see Shepard) - But our boundaries between ourselves and other
animals constantly shift and are not necessarily
rational. For instance, we are obsessed with
some species of animal (e.g. we construct
cemeteries for our pets) but fear and dislike
other species (e.g. animal phobias)
4Humans are unique, rational, cultural, moral,
powerful
Animals are subhuman, irrational, instinctual
or unconscious, amoral, powerless/dependent
- Utilitarianism animals are resources to be
consumed - Hierarchical(ism) animals have less value than
humans - Negativism animals are dangerous or unclean
- Dualism reason seen as most important
characteristic, leading to - Mechanism non-rational creatures lack thought
or feeling
5Humans and animals share basic bodily, mental,
and emotional experiences
- Engagement with scientific questions about
animal awareness and welfare - Broader definition of values animals not just
resources - Empathic engagement with animal experience and
emotion - As boundaries become less fixed, possibly less
appreciation of differences and inappropriate
analogising from humans to animals
(anthropomorphism)
6Drawing boundary lines what is natural?
- Around 12,000 yrs BCE large change when early
humans largely replaced or supplemented hunting
and gathering with agriculture and domestication
of animals - By 4000 BCE all the most important plants and
animals were domesticated - Studies of hunters/gatherers show an attitude of
reverence and respect for hunted animals and the
usage of rituals to engender respect and reduce
over-exploitation - Tendency in modern period to see this as
primitive and anthropomorphic
7Great chain of being scale of progression
from God to humans to animals (anthropocentric
vision)
Reason (uniquely human) essentially separate from
the body (largely animal)
Modern western view largely dualistic
Cartesian separation of rational humans from
animal automata mechanistic conception of
animals as unable to experience thought, or
feelings
Moral constructions of good and bad animals
most like humans are better than those who are
less humanlike or fit less into human social
structures
8Implications of the dualistic view
- Scientific difficulty to engage with questions
about animal mind and experience notion of a
mental rubicon (e.g. language, tool-use,
empathy, abstract thought) that separates humans
and animals - Contested and ambiguous classifications of
animals on a scale from most human to least human
9Implications of mechanistic view of animals
- I shall assume that no one would seriously
maintain that dogs, cats, sheep, cattle, pigs, or
chickens consciously think to themselves(p265).
Similarly then in the case of brutes since their
experiences, including their pains, are
nonconscious ones, their pains are of no
immediate moral concern. (Carruthers, 1989, p268
in Mitchell, Thompson Miles, 1997, p324) - The pig breeder aims to produce the largest
number of weaners per sow per annum, the grower
seeks to get his pigs to slaughtering weight in
the shortest possible time, the transporter wants
the animals loaded and delivered with the minimum
delays, and the slaughterman is chiefly concerned
with increasing the rate at which he can kill and
butcher the pigs that arrive at his abattoir.
And at the end of the chain stands the spectre of
the voracious consumer, who is solely interested
in buying the highest quality meat and bacon for
the lowest possible price. The fact that this
principle also ensures that the livestock
involved are subjected to a lifetime of continual
deprivation, distress and discomfort seems to be
largely irrelevant, merely an unfortunate
by-product of the harsh, economic necessities of
life. And the minority of people who display
genuine moral concern for the welfare of farm
animals often seem to be regarded as either
stupid, sentimental or just plain crazy.
(Serpell, 1996
10An anthrozoological perspective
- Can help us to deconstruct and understand
accepted cultural categorisations about animals
and our relationships with them, e.g.
classifications of good and bad animals - Can help us to understand how scientific thought
is structured by philosophical and cultural
assumptions, e.g. early studies of primate social
behaviour differed according to whether
researchers were male or female.
11Important issues to think about
- Traditional (dualistic) notion of thought as
being rational, abstract representation of real
categories out there in the world categories
may not be so fixed, may be more dependent on
culture and bodily experience - Scientific and popular conceptions of animals
have changed (and continue to change) over time
and place and are shaped by (often invisible)
philosophical assumptions - We can examine conceptions of animals in
language, images (e.g. advertising), dreams, as
well as in scientific models and metaphors - In all our considerations the animal-human
relationship is the context from which we work
and shapes, and is shaped by, our constructions
of animals
12References
- Arluke, A. Sanders, C. (1996). Regarding
animals. Philadelphia Temple University Press.
Chapters 2 and 7 especially - Berman, M. (1990). Coming to our senses. Bantam.
Especially chapter 2 - Birke, L. (1994). Feminism, animals and science
the naming of the shrew. Buckingham OUP.
Chapters 2 and 3. - Carruthers, P. (1989). Brute experience. Journal
of Philosophy, 86, 258-269. - Dolins, F.L. (Ed.) (1999). Attitudes to animals
views in animal welfare. Cambridge CUP. Chapter
1. - Haraway, D. (1992). Primate visions gender,
race, and nature in the world of modern science.
London Verso. - Lakoff, G. Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we
live by. Chicago and London University of
Chicago Press. - Mitchell, R.W., Thompson, N.S. Miles, H. L.
(1997). Anthropomorphism, anecdotes, and animals.
New York State University of New York Press. - Serpell, J. (1996). In the company of animals a
study of human-animal relationships. Cambridge
CUP. - Shepard, P. (1997). The others how animals made
us human. Shearwater Books.