Title: Hot Cognition
1Hot Cognition
2Hot Cognition
- Mental processes driven by our goals and
feelings. - Motivations and affect can influence the type of
cognitive processes we evoke and the concepts and
rules we activate. - We will begin by discussing Motivations and
goals. Thursday we will turn our attention to how
affect influences information processing.
3Motivation
- Kunda (1990) wrote a very influential paper
called Motivated Reasoning in which she
specified several types of goals and how those
can influence the reasoning process and judgment. - She specified 2 main types of goals, Directional
goals and Accuracy Goals, either of which can at
times lead to biases in information processing. - In the book, she mentions another type of goal
too, a closure goal, to which we will return.
4Directional goals
- The goal of arriving at a particular conclusion.
- Evidence for these goals comes from several areas
of research, such as the self-serving bias
literature, outcome dependency, the role of
arousal, self affirmation, and so forth.
5SSB
- Greenberg et al (1985)
- Participants took an aptitude test.
- IV told they did very well, very poorly, or
received no feedback - Participants were then given an opportunity to
evaluate the test. - What happened?
6The Self-Serving Bias
- In general, people take credit for their
successes, but blame external factors for their
failures. This phenomenon is known as the
self-serving bias. - Many have supposed that this bias results from
motivations.
7Self Serving Bias?
- We engage in a lot of mental gymnastics to paint
ourselves in the best possible light. For
example, we - derogate negative feedback
- think we are better than average on positive
attributes - change the way we think of ourselves so that we
are in a better position to get a job - think we are responsible for our groups success,
etc.
8Why do we do this?
- There are 2 primary accounts
- Motivational (2 very different views)
- The SSB serves an intra-psychic repair function
(it helps us feel better about ourselves) - Many have argued that we engage in this bias
because we want to, or have a need to, feel good
about ourselves (especially if we currently do
not). - The SSB helps us save face following failure
experiences (self-presentational motives).
9Support for each
- Intra-psychic repair
- Weary (1980) individuals report higher egotism
after claiming credit for success or denying
credit for failure. (suggest self-esteem was
enhanced). - Pyszczynski et al. (1985)
- Self-presentation
- Arkin (1979) SSB enhanced when participants are
highly publicly identifiable cf. control
(although still biased then). No results for
self-monitoring though, curiously.
10Why do we do this?
- 2. Cognitive
- We expect to do well (we are super awesome after
all) and we think we are good and smart). So, if
we do poorly, we conclude that the task (e.g., a
test) must be faulty, not that we are dumb. - Miller and Ross (1975) wrote a PR offering this
explanation. - Support abounds (e.g., Lassiter Munhall (2001).
11Which one is it? ORIs the SSB Multiply
Determined?
- Suppose you do poorly on a test, then blame the
professors unfair questions for your
performance. Do you feel better now? - Some evidence suggests that you would report that
you do (e.g., Weary, 1980). - However, such reports are the other half of the
SSB (i.e., the test is bad, I am good). - Self-presentational possibilities If you deny
credit for poor performance, it is inconsistent
to report feeling bad or having low Self Esteem.
12Study 1 Handley Carroll (in prep) Self-serving
or face saving?
- Study 1 was designed to test the idea that
engaging in the SSB after receive negative
feedback does not actually make individuals feel
better than individuals who are given no
opportunity to engage in the SSB. - We do derogate negative feedback though.
- Why?
- To save face perhaps to preserve our social
standing (we dont want to look like an idiot,
even if we know we are one). And, excuses work
(e.g., Schlenker).
13Our First Study Does the self-serving bias
actually serve the self?
- Participants took an IQ test, then were subjected
to one of 3 conditions - Negative feedback, evaluated the test.
- Negative feedback, did NOT evaluate the test.
- No feedback (control), evaluated the test.
- Then participants were told they were going to
watch either a Self-Enhancing video or a Neutral
video. - DV how much do you want to watch the video?
14Results Study 1
how much do you want to watch the video?
sig
sig
ns
15Study 1 Conclusions
- The SSB apparently does not make people feel
better about themselves. - These results are not clearly explained by the
cognitive account. - So, perhaps individuals derogate negative
feedback to save face.
16Study 2Does the SSB Help Us Save Face?
- A yoked experiment.
- Participants saw the test participants completed
in Study 1 (did not take the test) - Participants then saw actual test evaluations
from participants in Study 1, and their supposed
test performance, depending on condition. - Test, neg feedback, test evaluation
- Test, neg feedback, NO test evaluation
- Test, NO feedback, test evaluation (which were
not completed in a self-serving way)
17DVs for Study 2
- Aptitude, Competence, Intelligence
- And several others (all worked, significantly)
- Participants in Study 1 who received negative
feedback and answered questions in a self-serving
way were rated higher than participants from the
control condition and participants who received
negative feedback but did not get to answer
questions in an SS way.
18Results Study 2
Intelligence of Study 1 participants
sig
19Conclusions from Study 2
- Engaging in the self-serving bias DOES help us
save face in front of others. - This adds further support to the idea that we
engage in the SSB to save face, not to feel
better about ourselves.
20Outcome Dependency
- When your welfare or goals are tied to the
performance of another person, we are motivated
to believe they will do well. - Berschied et al. (1976)
- Had participants watch a videotape of three
individuals interacting, with one of whom the
participants believed they would be later going
on a blind date. - IV 4 groups, one thinking they would date a
different one of the 3 people, and a control
condition) - DV likeability and positive personality of each
person - Results?
21Outcome Dependency
- There is not really a non-motivational account
for these types of findingsmotivation seems
necessary to explain them. - Why? Participants had no prior expectation
about the people they saw, and the cognitive
account relies on the presence of such
expectations. - The information about the people was not varied,
just the knowledge they would be interacting with
a given person later.
22Arousal
- Cognitive Dissonance Theory (Festinger, 1957)
- We have a need for our attitudes, beliefs, and
behaviors to be in sync with each other that is,
to be consistent. - When we have incompatible attitude or beliefs, or
behave in a way inconsistent with or attitudes or
believes, we experience dissonance, a state of
negative arousal. - We experience dissonance because holding
inconsistencies imply that our future is
uncertain, that we cannot predict our own
attitudes and behaviors, and that we do not
understand ourselves. - When we experience dissonance, we are motivated
to reduce it (it is aversive).
23Arousal
- There are a few ways one can reduce dissonance
once they behave inconsistently with their
attitude - One can make the inconsistencies seem trivial
(its not that bad the way I do it) - One can rationalize the inconsistency (this is
just one bad behavior among many good ones) - One can change their attitudes to be in line with
their behavior. Now there is no inconsistency.
24Festinger and Carlsmith
- Peg-turning study
- Very boring task
- Paid 1 or 20 to lie to the next participant
that the experiment was actually a lot of fun. - 1 not enough to lie (Belief I do not lie for
a mere dollar) - 20 enough to lie (I think it is OK to tell a
little lie for 20). - 1 people, therefore, behaved inconsistently with
their belief. - So, what do they do regarding their attitudes
toward the task?
25Another example
- Write a pro-Castro essay
- IV told (forced) to write it or asked to write
it with strong encouragement (still, free will) - Forced belief that I would write a
counter-attitudinal essay when an authority
figure tells be too. - Free will I would prefer not to write a
counter-attitudinal essay (but they still do). - What happens?
26Self-Perception Theory (non-motivational)
- Bem (1967, 1972)
- We do not generally have direct access to our
attitudes. Rather, we look to our behavior to
figure out what our attitudes are, assuming they
are consistent with our behaviors - I wrote a pro-Castro because I was forced to, I
must not like Castro. - I wrote a pro-Castro essay of my own free will, I
must think Castro is an ok guy. - Detached outside observers would make the same
conclusions.
27Motivation or no?
- Enter Zanna and Cooper (1974)
- For negative arousal (dissonance) to motivate
attitude change - Individuals must experience the negative arousal
(i.e., dissonance actually exists) - Individuals must believe that arousal resulted
from behaving inconsistently with their
attitudes/beliefs.
28Zanna and Cooper (1974)
- In an experiment, all participants were asked to
ingest a pill (actually sugar placebo). - IV 1/3 told it would have no effect. 1/3 told it
would make them feel aroused and tense, 1/3 told
it would make them feel calm and relaxed. - Participants then wrote a counter-attitudinal
essay. - DV Participants reported their attitude about
the topic of the essay. - Results?
- Who reported attitude more favorable toward the
topic of the essay (i.e., who agreed with what
they wrote)?? - Control vs. aroused
- calm compared to others?
29Zanna and Cooper (1974)
- If participants were simply inferring their
attitudes as Bem suggested, their attributions
about the source of their arousal should not
matter. - Also, apparently participants did experience
arousal which had to be explained away otherwise
lead to attitude change. Bem suggested no arousal
would be evoked (or was necessary to explain the
results).
30Self-Affirmation
- Steele (1988)
- Engaging in counter-attitudinal behaviors leads
to attitude change because they threaten our
self-image. - That is, we feel bad about ourselves for lying,
writing pro-Castro essays, and so forth. - If I agree with what I just did, then I am not a
bad person after all. - But, we can feel better about ourselves via other
means as well. We can change our attitude to feel
better, or we could simply think about how great
we are in general. - If we find some other way to feel good about
ourselves, we will no longer experience a reason
to change attitude. - We are good again, so no need to change.
31Steele and Liu (1983)
- SV Recruited 2 types of students, some concerned
about economics and politics, and those who were
not. - All participants wrote an essay favoring a
tuition increase (counter). - IV ½ forced ½ freely chosen.
- Some participants were asked to report their
attitudes immediately. - Forced no change
- Free change (just like earlier experiments)
32Steele and Liu (1983)
- Some participants were asked to complete a
questionnaire about their political and economic
attitudes (i.e., self-affirm). - Irrelevant
- Forced no change
- Free change
- Self-affirmed
- Forced no change
- Free no change
33Bounded by reality?
- Our ability to engage in these biased directional
searches is bounded by reality. There is an
influence, but we are not completely out of
touch. - Remember the Santioso, Kunda, and Fong (1990)
work on introverts and extroverts. - Dunning et al (1989) better than average works
best for ambiguous traits, well defined ones like
punctual and well-read less so. See also Alicke
et al (1995) who find that the presence of others
reduces this effect.
34(No Transcript)
35Doosje et al. (1995)
- Doosje et al. (1995) good test question.
- IV Your group tends to be very, or not very,
pro-social. - IV This evidence is based on a large or small
sample. - People accepted positive and negative information
when the sample was large (rational) - People only accepted the positive conclusion when
the sample size was small. - Schaller (1992)
- There is a correlation between gender and
leadership. - Woman, but not men, were more likely to notice
that this correlation was an illusion due to the
fact that there are more men in the high level
executive positions.
36- Motivation can effect which rules we use
(representativeness, statistical, availability,
positive-test strategy, etc.) - It can also affect our effort.
37Lord, Ross, and Lepper (1979)
- Recruited Pro- and anti- capital punishment
people - Had them read one experiment that demonstrated
capital punishment was ineffective and one
experiment that demonstrated capital punishment
was effective in deterring crime. - Strength of the experiments were balanced.
- Participants reported their attitudes about
capital punishment after. - Subsequent research demonstrated this was due to
greater refutation (thoughts) about the counter
position. - Taylor -The biases that result from motivated
reasoning might be a good thing in that they keep
us happy, motivated, etc. Depressed people show
these biases less.
38Accuracy Goals
- Sometimes we adopt a goal to arrive at the best,
most accurate decision. - Accountability
- Importance
- When this is the case, we tend to think more
carefully, and invest more cognitive effort in
our decision making process. - However, this does not always mean that were will
arrive at the most accurate conclusion. - We may mistakenly use the wrong strategy, but
believe it will give us the correct answer.
39Accuracy Goals
- Tetlock and Kim (1987)
- Participants received information on 3
individuals. - Some participants were made accountable (must
explain decisions to research) or not. - Participants were asked to write down
descriptions, predict personalities of these 3
people. - Results?
- So, accuracy goals to seem to foster more?
40Accuracy Goals
- Kruglanski et al (1983, 1985) found that accuracy
goals also reduced several cognitive biases, such
as stereotype use, primacy effects, and
anchoring. - Sometimes, accuracy goals will result in the
adoption of faulty strategies, such as the
availability heuristic, or flawed statistical
reasoning (e.g., overlooking the base rates),
because we do not know the proper way to make the
judgment. - For example, people make less extreme judgments
of people when they are given irrelevant
information and accuracy goal (greater dilution
effects Tetlock Boettger, 1989).
41Tying together Directional and Accuracy Goals
- We engage in a decision-making process that
entails steps. This is Gollwitzers idea in a
nutshell (actually more elaborate) - 1) we need to make a decision evokes a
deliberative mindset in which we consider all
possibilities and try to arrive at the best
conclusion (accuracy goals). - 2) We make a decision, and need to carry it
through evokes an implementational mindset in
which we think of actions and thoughts needed to
bring our decision into reality (directional
goals). - See book for more.
- Closure goals see book.
42Approaching pleasure and avoiding pain (Higgins,
1997)
43Compatibility of strategy and task-framing
- Shah, Higgins, Friedman, 1998
- Anagram task In all cases, if solve 90 of the
anagrams, get paid 5 - HOWEVER, manipulate how this task is described to
participants. Task is framed to highlight - promotion (gain-frame) given 4, told that get
extra an extra 1 for solving 90 or more - prevention (loss-frame) given 5, told that can
avoid losing 1 if solve 90 or more
44Results
- No overall effect of task-framing on performance
- Performance was affected by the match between
participants chronic strategies and the task
framing - The stronger a participants promotion focus, the
better they did when the goal was framed in terms
of achieving a gain (e.g., earn an extra if
solve 90) - The stronger a participants prevention focus,
the better they did when the goal was framed in
terms of avoiding a loss (e.g., avoid losing a
if you solve 90)
45Promotion and prevention
- Some people are prevention vs. promotion focused.
- We can temporarily manipulate this construct
overall (across all individuals) - Influence our goals, choices, etc.
- Pills that reduce food consumption or boost
metabolism.
46Affect
47Mood-congruent Judgments
- Under many situations, our judgments will be
influenced in the direction of our affect. - Isen et al. (1978)
- IV Half of the participants received a gift,
half none. - Participants were later approached by another
experimenter and asked to rate various consumer
products. - Results.
48Mood-congruent Judgments
- Forgas (1994)
- IV Approached people after they watched a happy,
sad, or no movie. - DV Asked them to judge their own responsibility
for current conflicts in their intimate
relationships. - Results?
49Mood-congruent Judgments
- Bower (1981) network model, suggests that the
experience of affect can activate concepts
related to that affective valance. - Happy, happy concepts have an overall higher
level of activation.
50Mood-as-information
- Schwarz and Clore (1983)
- When making a judgment, our current affect can
serve as informationwhen relevant. - However, we often do not reflect carefully enough
on the relevance of the affect. - IV Called people on rainy or sunny days
- IV were first asked about the weather or not.
- DV report general life satisfaction.
- Results?
51Mood-as-information
- This highlights that the effects of affect on
judgment are not just influenced by spreading
activation (Bower). - If so, then we should see the same trend
regardless of the weather reminder. - But, when the informational value of affect is
compromised, it will not be used in judgments.