Diagnosis and style - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 72
About This Presentation
Title:

Diagnosis and style

Description:

Asks close-ended questions and focuses on first problem mentioned. ... A hard glossy coating. The rump or back part. Agent to treat bacteria. Secretion of body tissues ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:57
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 73
Provided by: garys7
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Diagnosis and style


1
Diagnosis and style
  • Health Psychology

2
The Practitioners Behaviour
  • Physicians tend to use a consistent style.
  • Two styles
  • Doctor-centered
  • Asks close-ended questions and focuses on first
    problem mentioned.
  • Ignores attempts to discuss other problems
  • Patient-centered
  • Asks open-ended questions and allows discussion
  • Avoids jargon and encourages participation in
    decisions

3
Haug and Lavin (1981)
  • Three conclusions
  • Both expressed a desire for the patient to
    participate in making decisions but this didn't
    happen very often.
  • If the patient wants to take part in decision
    making, but the doctor wants to make all the
    decisions, without finding out the patients
    opinion, then there is much conflict. Patient
    often told to find another doctor.
  • If the patient wants the doctor to make all of
    the decisions, but the doctor wants participation
    then this causes the patient to feel
    uncomfortable.

4
(Woodward and Wallston, 1987).
  • The elderly are more likely to prefer having
    decisions made for them (Woodward and Wallston,
    1987).

5
Patient input
  • If the doctor allows the patient to have an input
    into the decision making process then the patient
    will better adjust to the treatment regimen. the
    patient would be more satisfied with the
    treatment as well. (Auerbach, et al 1983,
    Martelli et al 1987).
  • Patients recover faster as well. (Brody et al
    1989, Mahler Kulik, 1991).

6
What do we want in a doctor?
  • Competency
  • Expertise
  • Concern, warm, sensitivity
  • How do good doctors benefit?
  • Patient is more adherent to treatment
  • Obtain more extensive diagnostic information

7
How do patients impair communications?
  • Not indicating distress
  • Poor communication of symptoms

8
Why do people describe their symptoms differently?
  • Symptom perception and interpretation
  • Differing common sense models of illness
  • Emphasizing or down-playing symptoms
  • Difficulties in communicating (e.g., language)

9
Kessler et al 1999
  • Kessler, D. A., Lloyd, K., Lewis, G., Gray, D.
    P., 1999, 'Cross-sectional study of symptom
    attribution and recognition of depression and
    anxiety in primary care', British Medical
    Journal, 318,7181,436-46
  •  
  • Aim
  • To investigate how a patient's style of behaviour
    can intervene in the doctor-patient relationship
    to the extent that it results in misdiagnosis.

10
Kessler et al 1999
  •  
  • Participants
  • 305 patients (225 women, 80 men), aged 16-90
    years (mean age 44 years), from a GP surgery in
    Bristol, consisting of eight doctors. Patients
    who attended both daytime and evening surgeries
    were included and were drawn from each of the
    eight doctors' panels. Informed consent was
    obtained and 26 patients declined taking part in
    the study. 24 participants failed to complete the
    surveys, so their data was discarded.

11
Kessler et al 1999
  •  
  • Procedure
  • Prior to their appointment with their GP,
    participants were asked to complete two
    questionnaires. The first was a 12-item general
    health questionnaire, which has been validated as
    a measure of psychological disorders. In
    particular, it is a valid tool for identifying
    the presence of depression and anxiety, where a
    score of three or more indicates the respondent
    has symptoms related to these two disorders.

12
Kessler et al 1999
  • The second questionnaire was the symptom
    interpretation questionnaire, which consists of
    13 common physical symptoms, accompanied by three
    possible causes, one from each of three
    categories. Depending on the number of choices
    made from each category (seven or more from one
    category), the participants were classified as
    having one of three attributional styles
    psychologizing, somaticizing and normalizing.
    Both questionnaires are self-administered.

13
Kessler et al 1999
  • Following this they were seen by their GPs, but
    told not to discuss the questionnaires with them.
    At the end of the surgery, the doctors, who were
    blind to which attributional category the
    patients were in, were asked to identify which
    patients they had noted as showing anxious and/or
    depressive symptoms and whether or not this was a
    new diagnosis.
  •  

14
Kessler et al 1999
  • Comparisons of the doctors' diagnoses with the
    patients' attributional style found that doctors
    were far more likely to identify psychologizers
    as having depressive/ anxious symptoms and far
    less likely to identify the same symptoms in
    normalizers. Thus the patients' way of thinking
    about their health (their attributional style)
    can affect the way they interact with their GP
    and, therefore, the diagnosis that is given.
  •  

15
Wallston (1978)
  • Information given first by the patient influences
    the doctor the most. Wallston (1978) found that
    doctors distorted the information that was given
    later in the consultation so that it fitted in
    with the diagnosis they made in the earlier part.

16
Korsch et al (1968)
  • Korsch et al (1968) found that a quarter of
    mothers attending a paediatric (child) clinic
    failed to tell the doctor their major concerns.

17
Weinman (1981)
  • Weinman (1981) choice of hypotheses affected by
  • The doctor's approach to health - psychological,
    biological or social explanations.
  • The probability of having a certain disease
  • The seriousness of the disease and its
    treatability. Easy treatment and life threatening
    if left untreated? Then go ahead with treatment!
  • Knowledge of the patient - Does the patient have
    a medical history of a certain type of illness?
    Do they go to the doctors often?

18
There are cultural differences in the
manifestation of symptoms
  • People from non-European cultures may well
    exhibit symptoms of their illness in a fashion
    that is quite strange to Europeans.
  • Torkington (1991) reports a case of a black man
    who had severe leg pains and convulsions.

19
There are cultural differences in the
manifestation of symptoms
  • Doctors found nothing wrong with his legs, and
    therefore placed the man in a psychiatric ward.
  • The symptoms of the patient were not recognised,
    when the patient really was suffering from
    physical distress.

20
The Practitioner's behaviour and style
  • Physicians can be doctor-centred or
    patient-centred. (Byrne and Long 1976).
  • 2,500 tape recorded medical consultations in
    several countries including England, Ireland,
    Australia and Holland.

21
The Practitioner's behaviour and style
  • Most styles were doctor-centred.
  • Physicians asked questions that required only
    brief replies (e.g. yes no, etc.).
  • Focus on first symptom or problem that was
    reported by the patient.
  • Often ignored attempts by patient to mention
    other symptoms.

22
The Practitioner's behaviour and style
  • Patient-centred approach -
  • doctors ask open-ended questions, requiring the
    patient to give lengthy replies.
  • Medical jargon was avoided.
  • They allowed patients to participate in the
    decision making process.

23
Memory
  • Ley et al (1973) found that information given in
    a structured way was better remembered than if
    given in an unstructured way.

24
Memory
  • 25 more information was remembered.
  • Students remembered 50 more information.
  • The experiment involved list learning, so was not
    ecologically valid.

25
Ley (1988)
  • Ley (1988) in a more ecologically valid
    experiment asked patients to recall what had been
    said in a real consultation.
  • 55 was remembered.

26
Ley (1988)
  • The following patterns in the errors made by the
    patients was found
  • they remembered the first thing they had been
    told (primacy effect)
  • the more information that was given, the less the
    patient remembered
  • repetition by the doctor had no effect
  • they remembered categorised information
  • they remembered more information, if they already
    had some medical knowledge.

27
Ley (1988)
  • A follow up study found that if doctors had read
    a booklet on how to communicate more clearly,
    then their patients remembered 70 of the
    information given to them.

28
Style
  • Patients prefer the doctor to show competence,
    sensitivity, warmth, and concern. (Ben-Sira,
    1980).
  • Patients take into account words, and body
    language - facial expressions, eye contact and
    body positions (DiMatteo, 1985).
  • Patients rate physicians who show little emotion
    less positively

29
Style
  • Open, approachable doctors are given more
    information by their patients.
  • The first complaint or detail a patient gives is
    often not the most significant.
  • Patients like a chance to be able to express
    themselves.

30
Style
  • They like clear explanations.
  • They like the doctor to show concern, and to give
    reassurance.
  • More sensitive doctors had less cancellations of
    appointments (DiMatteo et al, 1986).

31
The Patients Behaviour that Upsets the Doctor
  • Expressing anger or criticism
  • Ignoring or not listening
  • Insisting on procedures the physician thinks is
    not necessary
  • Requesting the doctor certify something he/she
    does not think is true (e.g., disability)
  • Sexually suggestive remarks

32
Doctors in the dock
  • Poor relationships between patients and doctors
    can increase the number of court cases against
    doctors for malpractice.
  • This in turn leads to more dissatisfaction with
    their career amongst doctors, also doctors become
    more wary of patients (Kolata, 1990).

33
Doctors in the dock
  • Many court cases allege that doctors did not
    communicate important information to their
    patients.

34
(Bishop Converse, 1986).
  • People may only communicate the points that they
    feel are important according to their notion of
    what is important about a particular complaint.
    (Bishop Converse, 1986).

35
Hypochondriacs
  • Hypochondriacs will overemphasise the symptoms,
    whereas another patient might play down their
    symptoms, in the hope that the physician will
    agree there is not much wrong with them.

36
Language differences
  • Language differences may impair communication.
  • This is a particular problem with young children,
    and people who cannot speak the language of the
    country.
  • Descriptions tend to be inaccurate or incomplete
    (Marcos et al, 1981).

37
Medical Terms Meaning Match terms to meanings
  • A hard glossy coating
  • The rump or back part
  • Agent to treat bacteria
  • Secretion of body tissues
  • Sugar produced by the body
  • The navel
  • A device to join separated tissue or bone
  • Substance that makes up plant or animal tissue
  • Antibiotics
  • Breech
  • Enamel
  • Glucose
  • Mucus
  • Suture
  • Protein
  • Umbilicus

38
Why Physicians Use Jargon
  • Habit
  • Patient doesnt need to know
  • Patient better off not knowing
  • Keep interactions short
  • Reduce emotional reactions
  • Reduce recognition of errors
  • Elevate practitioners status
  • Not aware of jargon

39
medical jargon
  • The doctor may use medical jargon, that is not
    understood by the patient.
  • Most patients, particularly those from less
    educated backgrounds fail to understand terms
    such as mucus', sutures' and glucose'.
    (DiMatteo DiNicola, 1982, McKinley 1975).

40
medical jargon
  • McKinlay (1975), study to see whether women in a
    maternity ward would understand 13 medical terms.
  • Two-thirds understood "breech" and "navel".
  • Almost none understood "protein" or "umbilicus".
  • On average each word was understood by 39 of the
    patients.

41
Patient dissatisfaction
  • Ley (1989) 21 surveys, 41 of patients
    dissatisfied with information given by hospital
    doctors.
  • 28 of patients dissatisfied with information
    given by general practitioners.

42
Patient dissatisfaction
  • Much of this is owing to the patients not
    understanding the doctors, or forgetting what
    they were told.
  • Patients also were reluctant to ask questions.

43
Boyle (1970)
  • Boyle (1970) 42 of patients cannot identify
    position of heart, 20 the stomach, and 49 the
    liver.

44
Bourhis, Roth and MacQueen (1989)
  • This study looks at the complexity of language
    used in hospitals and finds that whereas nurses
    are prepared to use everyday language as well as
    medical language doctors prefer medical language.
  • The medical language acts to increase the status
    and power of the doctors -

45
Aim
  • Bourhis et al were interested in finding out what
    factors affect communication between hospital
    staff and their patients.
  • Their aims were to examine the relationship
    between

46
Aim
  • the use of language between health professionals
    and their patients
  • the motivation either to change or to maintain
    the type of language used
  • the norms of communication in a hospital, and
  • the status and power differences that categorise
    patients, doctors and nurses.

47
Method
  • The study was carried out using three groups of
    respondents
  • 40 doctors,
  • 40 student nurses
  • and 40 patients.
  • All respondents were asked to complete a written
    questionnaire about the use of medical language
    (ML) and everyday language (EL) in the hospital
    setting.

48
Method
  • The questionnaire consisted of 4 sections.
  • The first section asked about the amount of
    medical and everyday language the respondent used
    in the hospital with members of the other groups
    in the study.
  • The second section asked the respondent to
    estimate how much ML and EL other members of
    their own group used with the other groups in the
    study.

49
Method
  • The third section asked the respondent to
    evaluate (on a 7-point scale) the appropriateness
    of the use of ML and EL among the study groups in
    the hospital setting.
  • The fourth section asked the respondents for
    background information and about their attitudes
    to various communication issues in the hospital.

50
Results
  • Doctors self-reports of their efforts to use EL
    with their patients were confirmed by other
    doctors but not by patients or nurses.
  • Patients self-reports stated that they
    themselves used EL, although those with limited
    knowledge of ML used this to try to communicate
    better with doctors.

51
Results
  • Doctors, however, did not encourage the use of ML
    by their patients, and reported the strongest
    preference of all the groups for patients to use
    EL.
  • Nurses were reported to have a very particular
    role by all three groups in their use of both EL
    and ML.

52
Results
  • They were seen as communication brokers between
    the EL of the patient group and the ML of the
    group of doctors.
  • The nurses were perceived as being able to
    mediate between the doctors and their patients.
  • All three groups agreed that EL was better for
    use with patients, and that use of ML often led
    to difficulties in communication.

53
So why is it that many doctors do not improve
their interpersonal skills?
54
Taylor (1986)
  • Taylor (1986) suggests that many doctors have not
    been trained in communication skills, because of
    three reasons
  • No general agreement as to what is a good
    consultation.
  • Good communication might make the doctor too
    sensitive to the needs of the patient and then
    cloud their medical judgement.
  • Doctors are too busy to be nice!

55
DiMatteo and DiNicola (1982)
  • DiMatteo and DiNicola (1982) point out that it is
    simple to address people by their name, say hello
    and goodbye , and to show them where to hang
    their coat.

56
(Thompson et al, 1990)
  • Patients could be given simple forms, whilst they
    are waiting to see the doctor.
  • They can write down any questions that they would
    like to ask the doctor in advance

57
Doctors get little feedback
  • Doctors get little feedback as to how successful
    their communication skills have been.
  • Is no news from the patient, an indication that
    they have been cured or have given up the
    treatment?

58
Computer Doctors
  • To get over the problem of embarrassment a
    computer could be used.
  • Robinson and West (1992) patients at a
    genito-urinary clinic (specialises in venereal
    disease) gave more information to a computer than
    they subsequently gave to the doctor.

59
Computer Doctors
  • Patients are less worried about social judgements
    and embarrassing details with a computer.
  • They admitted having more sexual partners,
    having attended before, and revealed more
    symptoms.

60
Schofield, M. J.. Walkom, S., Sanson-Fisher, R.,
1997,
  • Schofield, M. J.. Walkom, S., Sanson-Fisher, R.,
    1997, 'Patient-provider agreement on guidelines
    for preparation for breast cancer treatment',
    Hospital Topics, 75, 2, 18-27

61
Schofield, M. J.. Walkom, S., Sanson-Fisher, R.,
1997,
  • Aim
  • To investigate the level of agreement between
    doctors and patients and nurses and patients on
    guidelines needed to help prepare patients for
    breast cancer treatment. It was hypothesized
    that, due to their lack of interpersonal skills
    training, doctors would agree less with patients
    on the content and need for such guidelines than
    would nurses.

62
Schofield, M. J.. Walkom, S., Sanson-Fisher, R.,
1997,
  • Procedure
  • Each participant was sent a copy of the
    guidelines for preparing those with breast cancer
    for treatment and instructions on how to apply a
    rating scale to these guidelines. The guidelines
    were divided into two categories.

63
Schofield, M. J.. Walkom, S., Sanson-Fisher, R.,
1997,
  • The first concerned participants' attitudes to
    the importance of general principles about how
    patients should be prepared for potentially
    threatening medical procedures and contained 20
    items about such areas as medical practitioners
    giving patients time to ask questions and
    answering them fully, avoiding the use of jargon,
    being sensitive to the needs of the patient and
    so on.

64
Schofield, M. J.. Walkom, S., Sanson-Fisher, R.,
1997,
  • The second category contained items concerned
    with the specific steps that should be taken to
    prepare patients for such procedures, including
    the type of information and how it should be
    given before, during and after the treatment
    procedure.

65
Schofield, M. J.. Walkom, S., Sanson-Fisher, R.,
1997,
  • Example items from this category included asking
    the patient about the amount of detail they
    wanted to be given before the procedure and
    giving them an appropriate explanation of why
    that particular procedure had been chosen,
    summarizing what had been discussed, describing
    what was happening during the procedure and
    encouraging the patient to take an active part in
    the recovery phase, for example, by taking their
    own temperature.

66
Schofield, M. J.. Walkom, S., Sanson-Fisher, R.,
1997,
  • The following 5-point rating scale was used to
    measure the participants' attitudes to the
    questionnaire items 1 doctors should always do
    this, 2 desirable but not essential that
    doctors always do this, 3 not necessary for the
    doctor to always do this, 4 doctors should
    never do this, 5 not sure whether doctors
    should always do this.

67
Schofield, M. J.. Walkom, S., Sanson-Fisher, R.,
1997,
  • Results I
  • Overall, there was a high level of agreement
    about the nature of the guidelines, with only one
    from the first category (about patients viewing a
    video of the procedure before they underwent it
    themselves) and one from the second category
    (about asking patients about their previous ways
    of coping) not getting agreement from the
    majority of the three groups of participants.

68
Schofield, M. J.. Walkom, S., Sanson-Fisher, R.,
1997,
  • With regard to differences between the groups,
    doctors rated significantly fewer guidelines as
    important (i.e. scored them 1 or 2) than did
    nurses and patients. In particular, for the
    general principles they considered the provision
    of standardized written information to ensure
    consistency between staff, early preparation to
    allow time to practice for the recovery phase and
    the provision of additional ongoing support, for
    example by nurses, to be non-essentials.  

69
Schofield, M. J.. Walkom, S., Sanson-Fisher, R.,
1997,
  • In relation to the specific stages of
    preparation, doctors regarded such items as
    listening to the patients' concerns and tailoring
    information to meet them, asking them how much
    detail they would like on the procedure and
    teaching them specific coping strategies to be
    less important than did both nurses and patients.
    These results support the experimental
    hypothesis.

70
McDoctors
  • Perhaps we are getting too much into a McDonalds
    culture, where we expect a quick fix from the
    doctors using a limited range of treatments.

71
McDoctors
  • Ritzer (1993) - patients are now seen as
    customers or consumers.
  • Limited range of services, quick treatment -
    walk-in doctors.
  • Hospitals getting more commercial.

72
The end
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com