Title: DELUSIONS OF MISIDENTIFICATION: A COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHIATRY APPROACH
1DELUSIONS OF MISIDENTIFICATION A COGNITIVE
NEUROPSYCHIATRY APPROACH
- Hadyn D Ellis
- Cardiff University
- UK
- EllisH_at_cardiff.ac.uk
Athens, 2005
Advances in Psychiatry
2Cognitive Neuropsychiatry
- Cognitive Neuropsychiatry involves the use of
cognitive models to understand certain psychotic
symptoms. Further, where the models are
inadequate, they are modified to fit the data. - One of the appealing aspects of the Cognitive
Neuropsychiatric method is that it can generate
testable predictions. This contrasts with
traditional, post hoc psychodynamic approaches
that rarely, if ever, produce falsifiable
hypotheses.
Ellis, 1991 David 1993
3Delusional Misidentification Syndromes Paraprosopi
a faces seem horribly distorted Intermetamorpho
sis faces/objects change to look like
someone/something else Frégoli no change in
appearance but the belief that the person is
someone else in disguise Capgras belief that
people often, but not exclusively, close have
been substituted by impostor/robot. Capgras for
objects/places/animals also possible (cf.
Reduplicative Paramnesia)
4Paraprosopia
- Paraprosopia when a face appears to transform
within seconds into a grotesque mask, often
described by patients as a "monster", "vampire"
or "werewolf" Krauss, 1852. Most likely to be
reported by schizophrenic children but also
observed in adults (e.g. Daniel Paul Schreber,
1842-1911, President of the Court of Appeal in
Dresden, saw two men "as devils with particularly
red faces"). - AUTOPARAPROSOPIA paraprosopia for own face.
5Intermetamorphosis Courbon and Tusques (1932)
described Sylvie G, a 49-year-old woman who
claimed that objects and animals seemed
altered. People could change gender as she
looked at them. Many people looked like her son
or her aunt. She could distinguish them from her
true son only by examining their feet (his were
large and were invariably shod in dirty
shoes). Her husband might change appearance into
that of a neighbour (all except his eye colour
and missing finger). There were no further
reports of intermetamorphosis for 46 years, since
when five cases have been described, including
three by Young et al. (1990).
6The Frégoli Delusion
- First reported by Courbon and Fail (1927). They
described a 27-year-old woman, a domestic servant
with a passion for the theatre, who developed the
delusion that the actresses Robine and Sarah
Bernhardt were persecuting her in the guise of
others. - They suggested the term Frégoli delusion with
reference to the celebrated Italian mimic
Léopoldo Frégoli. - The essential feature of this delusion is that
there is no belief of any physical change
instead the patient believes that his/her
persecutors can invade the body of others. - We have no figures on the incidence of the
Frégoli delusion, except that it is more rare
than the Capgras delusion.
7(No Transcript)
8The Capgras Delusion
- First reported by Kahlbaum (1866) but more
extensively described by Capgras and colleagues
(1923, 1924). The essential symptom is that
patients assert that others usually, but not
necessarily, close to them have been replaced by
impostors, doubles, robots, etc. - Mme M, aged 53 years, believed husband, daughter
and many others had been replaced by doubles,
often many times. She also manifested delusions
of paranoia and grandeur. - The Capgras delusion is classified as a dangerous
delusion. It occurs in all societies and in
children as well as adults. It may be related to
the neurological syndrome REDUPLICATIVE
PARAMNESIA.
9The Psychodynamic Approach
- Capgras, and most who followed him, offered
various psychoanalytic explanations for the
Capgras delusion. - Enoch and Trethowan (1979) and Berson (1983)
identified specific problems whereby, in order to
deal with overwhelming, guilt-ridden ambivalence
about specific others, patients "split" into good
and bad (i.e. the double). - Problems
- (i) like all psychodynamic theories it is post
hoc and non-predictive - (ii) sometimes it is quite clear that there is
no prior ambivalence nor is the double
necessarily viewed in a negative way - (iii) although the ambivalence argument may be
stretched to misidentification delusions for
ANIMALS, it provides a less credible account of
MISIDENTIFICATION OF INANIMATE OBJECTS.
10Aetiology of DMS
Paranoid Schizophrenia Depression Cerebral
Infarction Brain Atrophy Tumours Alzheimers
Disease AIDS Encephalitis Migraine Epilepsy Diabet
es Toxic States Puerperal Psychosis Metabolic
Disorders Deficiency States (B12) Medications
11Simplified Modal Model of Face Recognition
12Simplified Modal Model of Face Recognition
Initial Structural Encoding
paraprosopia
Person Identity Nodes
13Simplified Modal Model of Face Recognition
14Simplified Modal Model of Face Recognition
15Capgras Right Hemisphere
Cutting (1985) - Capgras failure
in identification process, i.e., result of RH
dysfunction Bidault et al. (1986) - poor scores
on face processing tasks Feinberg Shapiro
(1989) - 26 cases, only two LH damage alone.
SPECT data during CD. An average 20 decrease in
uptake was observed in the right parietal
cortical area, with 11 left-right asymmetry
(normal lt 4)
(Lebert et al., 1994)
16Ellis et al. (1993) Tachistoscopic
Presentation of Pairs of Faces
Bilateral
LVF
RT
RVF
17Prosopagnosia
- Loss of recognition for once-familiar faces (but
not voices), usually following right
ventro-medial damage - Evidence of covert recognition
- SCR
- Face-naming priming
- Face interference
- Name re-learning
- Claim that these covert effects involve common
mechanism
18(No Transcript)
19SCRs to Familiar and Unfamiliar Faces Presented
Supraliminally and Subliminally
20Capgras as a Mirror-Image of Prosopagnosia
Limbic System
Visual Cortex
Bauers (1984) Dual Routes to Face Recognition
Limbic System
Visual Cortex
//
Prosopagnosia
//
Limbic System
Visual Cortex
Capgras Delusion
Ellis Young, 1990
21Prediction
- Capgras patients will not show the normally
appropriate skin conductance response to familiar
faces, despite the fact that these will be
overtly recognized.
(Ellis Young, 1990)
22Experiment
- 5 patients with Capgras delusion
- 5 matched patients with other delusions
- 5 controls
- Presented (randomly) with
- 5 famous faces
- 25 unfamiliar faces
23Mean range-corrected SCR amplitude to familiar
and unfamiliar faces for normal
controls,psychiatric controls and people with
Capgras delusion.
Adapted from Ellis et al. (1997) Proc. R. Soc. B
24Mean SCR amplitude (uS) across five presentations
of an auditory tone for normal controls and
people with Capgras delusion.Ellis et al.
(1997) Proc. R. Soc. B
25(No Transcript)
26Modified Face Recognition Model
(Ellis Lewis, 2001)
27CAPGRAS DELUSIONA Two-Stage Model
- Disruption/damage to the usual affective linkage
to familiar face recognition a sufficient
explanation? (cf. Maher, 1974) - Tranel et al. (1995) fronto ventromedial lesions
-gt no differential SCRs to familiar and
unfamiliar faces but no Capgras delusion - Additional stage, involving attribution seems
likely (Ellis Young 1996 Young de Pauw, 2002) - Attribution steered by paranoia (cf. Cotards
where delusion may result from depression)
(Young, 1997 Gerrens, 2000) - Also an inability to revise the bizarre
belief-despite evidence
28Summary
- The Cognitive Neuropsychiatric approach to
monothematic delusions can be successful - Delusions of misidentification may be explained
by a model of normal face recognition - However, for the Capgras delusion, the model
itself must be altered to include emotional
recognition of familiar others - For Capgras delusion to develop it may be
necessary for dysfunction to occur at
- a perceptual/cognitive level
- and
- (b) an attribution/judgement stage of decision
making
29THE END
30Capgras Syndrome A Dangerous Delusion
- Blount (1986) studied 50 schizophrenics in a
maximum security facility, three of whom
manifested symptoms of Capgras syndrome.
Two have killed persons, both brutally. One
bludgeoned and then decapitated his 82 year
old stepfather (explaining) he had been
searching for batteries and microfilm in his
head to take to court to prove he was a robot
p. 207