Title: How Does Age Affect Our Safety
1How Does Age Affect Our Safety?
- James A Thomson,
- University of Strathclyde
2- Early and middle childhood
- Late childhood and adolescence
- The later years
3Underlying Factors
- Knowledge
- Attitudes
- Skill
4Can Childrens Traffic Behaviour be Improved
Through Training?
- Sandels (1975) It is not possible to adapt
fully young children (under 10 years) to the
traffic environment. It are biologically
incapable of managing its many demands. - Björklid (1998) Children below the age of 12
lack the developmental apparatus needed for
consistently safe behaviour in traffic.
5Proposed Constraints on Child Development
- Unable to understand others perspectives
(Egocentrism) - Difficulty in making judgements that rely on
combining variables (e.g., speed and distance) - Difficulty in dividing attention between tasks
- Difficulty in perceiving causal relationships
6Piagets Stage Theory of Development
- Development proceeds by way of four main stages
- All stages must be passed through, and in a fixed
order. - The stages occur at approximately the same age in
all children and in all cultures - This has often led to the conclusion that the
development of cognitive functioning is
biologically determined and not readily affected
by learning. Children can therefore only be
taught certain things when they are ready
7Perspective Based on Misinterpretation of
Piagets (1955) Stage Theory
- Pre-operational stage defined as ending at age 7,
not 12 - Progress through the stages a matter of
experience, not biological maturation - Ages intended only as a rough guide
- Piaget much less wedded to the notion of uniform
developmental stages
8Evidence against Fixed Constraints (Whitebread
Neilson, 1996)
9Aspects of childrens thinking that need to be
addressed
- Meta-cognition (e.g., strategic thinking,
planning, self-monitoring) -
- Use of rules
- Generalisation and context dependency
10Solution
- Focus on behaviour and underlying skills
- Train in appropriate contexts
- Develop strategic thinking and conceptual
understanding - This can accelerate pedestrian skill development
by several years (e.g., Thomson Whelan, 1996)
11Limitations on Training
- Pedestrian training cannot raise performance to
100 safety - But aim of education should not be to turn out
finished articles, especially given the amount
of input that is feasible - This expectation would not be held about any
other educational programme
12More Realistic Goals
- Reduce probability of poor decisions by any
margin that is feasible. Any improvement is
valuable - In fact, small amounts of training can bring
about substantial improvements - Help children become safe and autonomous
learners. Training does not mean sudden freedom,
but continued supervision - Training should enable children to become more
attentive and aware supervisees - Prepare them for when they become unsupervised
as in driver education
13Conclusion
- Education has a distinctive and vital role to
play in reducing child pedestrian accidents. - Children as young as 5 years do benefit from such
education. But independent travel in the
short-term is not a realistic goal. Continued
supervision remains essential. - The aim should be to improve childrens safety in
the short-term by whatever amount is possible.
But educational objectives must be essentially
long-term.
14Pedestrian Safety in Adolescence
- A time of transition
- Parental versus peer group influence
- Norm-breaking
- Risk a good idea?
- Therefore attitudes probably of much greater
importance than in younger children
15Challenges for the Adolescent Pedestrian
- Exposure to traffic
- Complexity of the traffic environment
- Existing skill levels therefore under pressure
- Are those skill levels adequate?
16Skill Levels in Adolescence
- Competence of this group may be over-estimated
- Natural tendency to over-estimate own skill
(Svenson, 1981) - Illusion of control (McKenna, 1993)
17Conclusion
- Need to assess pedestrian skill levels in this
age group bearing in mind the more complex
traffic environment with which they will
interact. - Perceived behavioural control do children in
this age group over-estimate their skill more
than younger or older children? - Attitudes and social norms in this age group need
investigation.
18The older pedestrian
- Speed of information processing declines with age
(Salthouse, 1991) - - reaction time
- - speed/accuracy trade-off
-
- Increasing task complexity affects older people
disproportionately (Cerella, 1985) - - decision times
- - divided attention
- Effects of cognitive load more marked where task
is novel or unfamiliar (Rabbitt Maylor, 1991) - Lapses (Rabbitt, 1983)
19Compensating for the effects of ageing
- Many of these age effects disappear after
practice (Rabbitt, 1980) - The effects are less marked in highly skilled
activities (chess, bridge). Limited impact model
(Maylor, 1994) - Evidence of meta-cognitive awareness and strategy
adaptation - - trade off speed for accuracy
- - forward planning and anticipation
- - extra caution following errors
- Individual differences more marked than in
general population (Rabbitt, 1983)
20Conclusions
- Needs more investigation in the context of driver
and pedestrian behaviour - Importance of self-monitoring
- Possibility that adaptive strategies might be
promoted to overcome some of these problems