Title: Increasing Variance as a Function of Aging
1Increasing Variance as a Function of Aging
- Harvey Babkoff
- Elisheva Ben-Artzi
- Leah Fostick
- Miriam Geal-Dor
2Changes in Variance Due to Aging
- Changes in Mean and in Variance due to Aging
- Inter-Individual Variance and Aging
- Intra-Individual Variance and Aging
3- Inter-Individual Variance and Aging
4Lovden et al. (2004) and Schie (2000)
- Differences in physiological, socio-demographic
and educational factors become greater as
individuals age and therefore begin to have
greater weighting than the genetic factors in
determining cognitive performance.
5Quantitative Genetic Analysis of Latent Growth
Curve Models ofCognitive Abilities in Adulthood
Chandra A. ReynoldsUniversity of California,
Riverside (2005)
- Increased variability in cognitive performance
with age has been - reported primarily in, but has not been limited
to, cross-sectional - analyses (e.g., Christensen, Mackinnon, Korten,
Jorm, Henderson, - Jacomb, 1999 Christensen, Mackinnon, Korten,
Jorm, Henderson, Jacomb, Rodgers, 1999 Morse,
1993). In the present case, such a pattern was
seen with respect to systematic variances, that
is, those explained by the latent growth model,
for most measures. Variance increases have been
ascribed to non-shared environmental or
non-genetic stochastic processes (e.g., Finch
Kirkwood, 2000). This interpretation was
supported by our findings. The increasing
environmental variation seen for nearly all
cognitive traits in the present study could
reflect stochastic processes that may have their
seeds in early development and that are magnified
in late life (Finch Kirkwood, 2000).
6Quantitative Genetic Analysis of Latent Growth
Curve Models ofCognitive Abilities in
AdulthoodChandra A. ReynoldsUniversity of
California, Riverside (2005)
- Though many cognitive abilities exhibit marked
decline over the adult years, individual
differences in rates of change have been
observed. In the current study, biometrical
latent growth models were used to examine sources
of variability for ability level (intercept) and
change (linear and quadratic effects) for verbal,
fluid, memory, and perceptual speed abilities in
the Swedish Adoption/Twin Study of Aging. Genetic
influences were more important for ability level
at age 65 and quadratic change than for linear
slope at age 65. Expected variance components
indicated decreasing genetic and increasing
non-shared environmental variation over age.
Exceptions included one verbal and two memory
measures that showed increasing genetic and
non-shared environmental variance. The present
findings provide support for theories of the
increasing influence of the environment with age
on cognitive abilities.
7Examples of psychophysical data showing no
changes in either mean performance or variance as
individuals age
8An example of stable performance across age in
accuracy of discrimination of auditory target
(tone) from among non-target tones. There is no
significant change in performance either in the
mean or in the variance of the distributions as a
function of aging
9An example of stable performance across age in RT
to auditory target (tone) from among non-target
tones. There is no significant change in
performance either in the mean or in the variance
of the distributions as a function of aging
10Another example of stable performance across age
in RT to auditory target (phonological stimulus)
from among non-target stimuli. There is no
significant change in performance either in the
mean or in the variance of the distributions as a
function of aging
11(No Transcript)
12The following data are an example of increased
inter-individual variance in the elderly together
with an increased mean but with a scaling
problem and change in distribution
13Note what appears to be an increase in variance
from the young to the elderly subjects. This
seems, however, to reflect the significant
decrease in mean percent correct for the group of
elderly subjects relative to the younger subjects
and a resultant change in the distribution from
non-Gaussian to Gaussian.
14Note the changes in the accuracy distributions of
the young versus the elderly subjects. While the
distribution of the young is not Gaussian, that
of the elderly appears to approach a Gaussian
distribution.
15An example of change in performance as subjects
age in the accuracy of discrimination of a
semantic target (word) from among non-target
words. There is a significant change in mean
performance but not in the variance of the
distributions as a function of aging.
16There was a significant increase in the variance
of the RT distributions to the semantic targets
from the young to the elderly (F 2.8278
plt.016). Both distributions are Gaussian.
17The variance in the group aged 60 is
significantly larger than the variance in the
group aged 20-39. (F(1, 57) 5.66 plt.001). Both
distributions are Gaussian (See following slide).
18(No Transcript)
19(No Transcript)
20Some cross-sectional studies have reported
results that were interpreted to mean that
inter-individual variability among the elderly is
related to variability in hearing loss (Helfer
and Wilber, 1990 Humes et al., 1994). However,
the results of other studies have found
inter-individual variability to be relatively
large even among elderly subjects with normal
audiograms for their age (Brasz et al., 2002
Scneider Pichora-Fuller, 2001 Versfeld and
Dreschler, 2002).
21(No Transcript)
22(No Transcript)
23(No Transcript)
24Both the distributions of the hemispheric index
for the young and for the elderly are Gaussian.
There is a tendency for the variance of the
distribution of the hemispheric index of the
elderly to be larger than for the younger
subjects (F 1.985) plt.07
25(No Transcript)
26(No Transcript)
27(No Transcript)
28- Intra-Individual Variance and Aging
- Speech Discrimination in Speech Noise
29INTRA-INDIVIDUAL VARIANCE and AGINGIn recent
years there has been growing interest in
within-person variability as a potentially
informative individual difference parameter. At
least five factors may contribute to this
interest. (Correlates of within-person (across
occasion) variability in RT, Salthouse and
Berish,Neuropsychology, 2005)
- First, high levels of within-person variability
may signify health-related problems. - Second, unusual levels of within-person
variability might function as an early indicator
of impending cognitive change. That is, large
fluctuations in ones momentary level of
cognitive performance may be a precursor to
certain types of cognitive pathology, and
information about variability might provide a
more useful baseline against which to evaluate
the severity of extreme behaviors. - Third, large within-person variability (i.e., low
across-occasion consistency) could distort the
evaluation of an individuals level of cognitive
functioning. - Fourth, within-person variability could
contribute to inconsistency in research results
across studies because in a single-occasion
study, it is impossible to distinguish relatively
stable trait variance from fluctuating state
variance. For example, if there are age
differences in within-person variability, it
could lead to spurious conclusions of age-related
increases in between-persons variability
(Nesselroade, 2001). The possibility of large
within-person variability in the performance of
neuropsychological tests would also raise
questions about the basis of the correspondence
typically assumed between test performance and
brain function. - Fifth, identification of correlates of
within-person variability may be informative
about possible causes of individual differences,
and particularly age-related individual
differences, in cognitive functioning.
30(No Transcript)
31(Correlates of within-person (across occasion)
variability in RT. Salthouse and
Berish,Neuropsychology, 2005)
- There are two major results of these studies.
The first is the demonstration of very large
within-person variability in measures of RT. In
two studies involving a total of 420 individuals,
the median RT from one occasion to the next was
found to vary as much as the mean (across
occasion) RT varied among people who ranged from
18 to 91 years of age. Because the reliability
estimates indicate that for most participants the
within-occasion RTs were more similar to one
another than were the between-occasions RTs, this
across-occasion variability cannot merely be
attributed to - random fluctuation.
- The second major result of the present studies is
the consistent finding that measures of
within-person variability appear to be secondary
to measures of central tendency with respect to
relations with age and with a variety of
cognitive variables. For many variables, the mean
and the SD are highly related because the
variability around the mean is frequently greater
when the mean is larger, but all of the analyses
indicated that statistical control of the mean
has a greater attenuating effect on correlations
involving the SD than vice versa.
32(No Transcript)
33(No Transcript)
34(No Transcript)