Interpreting Age Effects in Second Language Acquisition - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 50
About This Presentation
Title:

Interpreting Age Effects in Second Language Acquisition

Description:

Many have contributed this to a critical period. ... A view of temporal and geometric features characteristic of critical periods ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:648
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 51
Provided by: Davi693
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Interpreting Age Effects in Second Language Acquisition


1
Interpreting Age Effects in Second Language
Acquisition
David Birdsong
  • A presentation of David Birdsongs chapter from
    the Handbook of Bilingualism Psycholinguistic
    Approaches edited by
  • Judith F. Kroll and Annette M.B. de Groot

David Counselman Final Project APLNG 591 SLA
December 11, 2006
2
Introduction
  • There is an undeniable, general correlation
    between age and skill at picking up a foreign
    language.
  • Many have contributed this to a critical period.
  • This is reasonable, considering some other
    evidence from science

3
Cont
  • For example, there is a timetable associated with
    song learning in some birds (Marler 1991)
  • Also, for vision in cats to develop fully,
    stimulation must be exposed to the visual cortex
    within the first three months of age (Hubel and
    Wiesel 1965)
  • However, some researchers arent convinced that
    language acquisition is subject to a similar
    critical period.

4
Outline
  • A general orientation to typical methodologies in
    SLA age factor research
  • A view of temporal and geometric features
    characteristic of critical periods
  • More specific data found within SLA research on
    the age problem
  • The question of native-like performance and how
    it relates to the slope of the age-function
  • Determining the slope of the age function
  • Concluding remarks

5
The Age Question in SLA Research
  • Often, as well as in this chapter, the age
    variable is the age of arrival (AoA) into the
    culture of the L2
  • The issue of the end state
  • Researchers usually set a bottom cut-off point
    for Length of Residence (LoR) like 10 years
  • Usually, the issue is the following
  • How does AoA affect L2 attainment or competency?

6
Cont
  • Specifically, these are run experimentally in the
    laboratory
  • subjects are asked to complete any of various
    tasks used to rate language competence.
  • Results vary depending on setting, methodology,
    etc.
  • Even presentation of material (orally vs
    visually) can make a difference (Bialystok and
    Miller 1999, Johnson 1992)

7
Cont
  • This brings up an important fact
  • The function of the effect of age will vary
    depending on the area of the L2 grammar being
    considered (e.g. Bialystok and Miller 1999,
    Birdsong and Flege 2001).
  • Linguistic feature, amount of L2 use, and L1-L2
    pairing have all been found to play a role as
    well (see Flege, Yeni-Komshian Liu 1999, Moyer
    1999, Scovel 1988, Seliger 1978)

8
  • Research has also looked at neurological
    dimensions of the issue, although these issues
    are not considered in detail in this article.
  • Event-related brain potentials, fMRI, and
    positron emission tomography, for example.
  • Also, do polyglots engage distinct neurological
    substrates in processing/ representing each
    language?
  • Some evidence converges to say that high levels
    of L2 attainment correlate with the use of a
    common neurofunctional organization for the L1
    and L2 (see Brovetto 2002 for further discussion
    of this issue).

9
Geometric and Temporal features of Critical
Periods
  • Generally, a critical period refers to a
    temperal span during which an organism displays
    a heightened sensitivity to certain environmental
    stimuli, the presence of which is required to
    trigger a developmental event.

10
  • Some people assume the degree of sensitivity is
    constant throughout this temporal span (as
    pointed out by Bornstein 1989 183), which would
    imply

11
This view will be taken here
  • Bornstein (1989) also noted, however, that a
    critical period, by definition, includes the time
    during its onset and offset, and so the function
    should look like this

12
A CP for language acquisition
  • Since sounds like ba and pa can be
    distinguished by newborns a few hours old (Eimas,
    Siqueland, Jusczyk Vigorito 1971), it can be
    argued that the onset essentially begins at
    birth.
  • Most accounts consider full adult-like competence
    to be possible if language learning begins by 4
    to 7 years of age. The beginning of the offset,
    could, therefore, be roughly located around this
    age.

13
Cont
  • It is argued (Johnson and Newport 1989) that
    critical period effects for language acquisition
    should end around the point of complete
    neurocognitive maturation, roughly in the
    mid-to-late teens.
  • This could be the end of the offset of the CP.

14
This provides the following
15
Other possible age-functions
  • No end point the decline in attainment of
    language continues throughout the lifespan.

All of these functions will be compared with the
CPH (Lenneberg 1967) and with observations found
in the SLA literature.
16
CPH in the context of SLA
  • The CPH has been formulated in various ways
  • Increasing age entails
  • Decreasing neural plasticity in the relevant
    cortical areas (Lenneberg 1967, Pulvermüller
    Shumann 1994)
  • Loss of access to the language learning faculty
    or universal grammar (Eubank Gregg 1999)
  • A gain in information processing capacity that
    might be ill-suited to the task of language
    acquisition (Newport 1991)
  • Dismantling of relevant cortical circuitry
    (Pinker 1994)
  • Loss of function of mediating mental faculties
    from lack of use (Bever 1981)
  • The inhibition of learning by entrenched L1
    knowledge (Marchman 1993)
  • See Birdsong (1999) for further discussion.

17
The function described by a CP
  • Pinkers proposal (1994 293-294) is the only one
    that makes claims for the end of the offset,
    thereby fitting the function for critical
    periods, which has already been shown
  • Pinker proposes that once the faculty to learn
    language has completed its task (once weve
    reached puberty and have learned our L1), it gets
    discarded to save metabolic energy. (liked to
    floppy disk)

18
  • Johnson and Newport (1989) make a similar
    proposal (which fits the geometric and temporal
    features of a CP), but without mention of the
    same set of neural and metabolic mechanisms as
    Pinker.
  • Other proposals do not predict a leveling off at
    the end of the critical period, and therefore
    cannot define a CP function, but well see that
    these proposals arent unworthy, they in fact
    seem to be even closer to reality.

19
Behavioral Data
  • The primary argument against the CPH is that the
    data regularly fail to show a leveling off that
    signals the end of the CP.
  • If language learning is related to maturation,
    there isnt any maturational difference in a 17
    y/o and a 27 y/o, so the CPH claims that people
    of both ages should reach the same level of
    attainment (Johnson and Newport 1989 79)

20
Straight-line Age Effects
  • Flege (1999) reanalyzed data from Flege et al.
    (1995) to show that there was no discontinuity in
    ratings of perceived foreign accent as
    a function of age.

21
Cont
  • Flege (1999) also reports that in Yeni-Komshian,
    Flege Liu (1997), there was also no evidence of
    a break in linearity.
  • Hakuta, Bialystok Wiley (2001) found the same
    type of linear function when analysing the 1990
    U.S. Census results, which included self reports
    of Chinese and Spanish immigrants English oral
    proficiency.

22
Cont
  • Bialystok and Hakuta (1999) looked at a subset of
    these immigrants who had LoR of at least 10 years
    and whose AoR ranged from 0 60
  • Again, theres a linear trend in the data
  • The results of these analyses

23
Chinese Spanish
24
Cont
  • Flege et al. (1999) also found no evidence for a
    discontinuity in accent ratings of 240 Korean
    immigrants
  • but DID find a slight discontinuity in their
    scores on a 144-item test on knowledge of English
    morphosyntax.
  • However, even late-arriving Koreans were subject
    to a negatively-sloping fuction of age.

25
Disaggregation Analyses
  • These analyses, in which age groups are divided
    at some age near puberty or a little after, often
    show a discontinuity in the slope of the age
    function.
  • Patkowski (1990), for example, analysed his 1980
    dissertation data by dividing his subjects into
    early (younger than 15) and older (older than 15)
    learner groups
  • Early arrivals regression line .052
  • Late arrivals regression line .028

26
Johnson Newport (1989)
  • Used this method
  • Results provided evidence in favor of the CPH
  • Birdsong and Molis (2001) replotted the JN89
    data and proved different results.
  • BM01 also replicated the JN89 study with
    Spanish natives, and obtained a totally observe
    pattern.

27
  • Here, the dotted line, JN89 shows a decline in
    early stages and a leveling off near the end
    (CPH)
  • But BM01s replication of the study shows
    different results, in which there is a ceiling
    effect for early learnings and then a continuous
    decline after age 16.
  • There is no patten here for the late arrivals.
  • While this doesnt provide evidence for what the
    function may look like, it definitely does not
    provide evidence for a leveling off and bottoming
    out of the function in late arrivals.

28
Cont
  • Also interesting, and important, is that
    Bialystok Hakuta (1994) reanalyse the JN89
    data
  • and find that by moving the age of arrival
    split to 20 years of age
  • the linear correlation of late arrivals
    increased to significance.

29
Summary from the data
  • Some studies show a steady continuous decline in
    language attainment as a function of age.
  • Others show that there is a discontinuity, where
    the slope for the late arrivals is much steeper
    than for early arrivals.
  • In either case, there is NO evidence for a
    flattening out of attainment, which rejects the
    notion of an endpoint to the decline of
    sensitivity at some age after puberty.

30
The functions we looked at
  • These results do not support the function
    associated with the CPH. Results that suggest a
    discontinuity look more like graph C, while other
    studies resemble the graphs A or B, depending.

31
Nativelike Attainment
  • In the experimental context, usually defined as
    falling within the range of native speakers
  • Some researchers strictly define that
    nativelikeness must fall within one standard
    deviation of native speakers.
  • Some argue (Long 1990) that if late learners
    achieve nativelike scores on experimental tasks,
    this provides evidence to refute the CPH.

32
Cont
  • In earlier research, evidence for this was not
    found.
  • In latter research, however, much evidence of
    this type has been found.
  • It is interesting though that most late learners
    who fall within nativelike ranges are usually
    earlier late learners (late teens).

33
Cautions should be considered
  • Both the native speaker and immigrant subjects
    should be taken from the general population, and
    not the cream of the crop
  • Some L2 speakers have had varying degrees of L2
    exposure prior to immigration.
  • The L2 sample should be controlled to make sure
    subjects are at or near the end-state. (2 years
    VS 25 years)

34
Other problems implications
  • It can also be problematic to compare any
    bilingual to a monolingual native-speaker group.
  • A bilingual is not two monolinguals in one
    (Grosjean 1989)
  • Previous studies that have claimed 5 or less of
    the population reaches native-like proficiency
    (Bley-Vroman 1989, Selinker 1972) may not have
    been sampling the right group of people.

35
Nativelike Attainment and the Age Function
  • It should be considered that the likelihood of
    nativelike attainment in latter ages is subject
    to the slope of the age function.
  • Steep slope implies less chance of nativelike
    attainment in older ages.
  • Shallower slope implies more chance.

36
Age and Other Factors
  • If the slope of the age function is so crucial to
    determining the likelihood of attaining
    nativelike competence
  • how can we determine the slope?
  • what affects the slope the function?
  • what exactly does the function look like?

37
Cont
  • Years of education have been found to play a role
    (Bialystok and Hakuta 1999)
  • Greater use of the L2 has correlated with less
    perceived foreign accent (Flege colleagues)
  • A variety of other cognitive, task-related,
    attitudinal, experiential, demographic, aptitude,
    and training variables may affect the slope as
    well.

38
Different aspects of linguistic knowledge
  • It is also important to note that the age
    function does not affect all areas of language
    competence the same (Eubank and Gregg 1999, Flege
    et al. (1999), Flynn and Manuel (1991), Seliger
    (1978), and others).
  • For example, Birdsong and Flege (2001) found that
    increasing AoA did not affect accuracy of
    knowledge of regular verb forms in English, but
    it did affect the knowledge of irregular verb
    forms.

39
(No Transcript)
40
Declarative VS Procedural Memory
  • Birdsong (2004) suggested that the effect of
    aging on declarative memory is likely a big issue
    here.
  • Declarative memory declines much more with age
    than procedural memory does.
  • This distinction should also be considered, when
    trying to understand the age function on language.

41
Conclusion
  • The observations observed in this chapter
    illustrate evidence that the age function does
    not match the predictions of the CPH
  • Degree of L2 attainment does not appear to be
    linked to maturational milestones, but rather
    with age in general.
  • The likelihood of native-like attainment
    decreases throughout the age spectrum (not does
    level off) progressively.

42
Cont
  • Not everyone experiences this decline to the same
    degree or at the same pace.
  • Whatever it is that allows language acquisition
    (brain plasticity?) is not one thing that affects
    the global mechanism for language learning
  • age affects the acquisition of various
    linguistic features differently (regular vs
    irregular, for example).

43
Closing remark
  • Flege opened and closed his plenary speech (age
    effects on L2A) at the Conference on Hispanic
    Linguistics and Acquisition by saying
  • I hope you now understand less about the effect
    that age has on language acquisition than you did
    before the presentation.

44
References
  • Bever, T. G. (1981). Normal acquisition
    processes explain the critical period for
    language learning. In K. C. Diller (Ed.),
    Individual differences and universals in language
    learning aptitude (pp. 176-198). Rowley, MA
    Newbury House.
  • Bialystok, E., Hakuta, K. (1994). In other
    words The science and psychology of
    second-language acquisition. New York Basic
    Books
  • Bialystok, E., Hakuta, K. (1999). Confounded
    age Linguistic and cognitive factors in age
    differences for second language acquisition. In
    D. Birdsong (Ed.), Second language acquisition
    and the critical period hypothesis (pp. 161-181).
    Mahwah, NJ Erlbaum.
  • Bialystok, E., Miller, B. (1999). The problem
    of age in second language acquisition Influences
    from language, task, and structure.
    Bilingualism Language and Cognition, 2, 127-145.
  • Birdsong, D. (1999). Introduction Whys and why
    nots of the critical period hypothesis. In D.
    Birdsong (Ed.), Second Language acquisitoin and
    the critical period hypothesis (pp. 1-22).
    Mahwah, NJ Erlbaum.

45
References
  • Birdsong, D. (2004). Second language acquisition
    and ultimate attainment. In A. Davies and C.
    Elder (Eds.), The handbook of applies linguistics
    (pp. 82-105). London Blackwell.
  • Birdsong, D., Flege, J. E. (2001).
    Regular-irregular dissociations in the
    acquisition of English as a second language. In
    A. H.-J. Do, L. Domínguez, A. Johansen (Eds.),
    BUCLD 25 Proceedings of the 25th Annual Boston
    University Conference on Language Development
    (pp. 123-132). Boston Cascadilla Press.
  • Birdsong, D., Molis, M. (2001). On the evidence
    for maturational effects in second language
    acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 44,
    235-149.
  • Bley-Vroman, R. (1989). What is the logical
    problem of foreign language learning? In S. Gass
    J. Schachter (Eds.), Linguistics perspectives
    on second language acquisition (pp. 41-68).
    Cambridge, U.K. Cambridge University Press.

46
References
  • Bornstein, M. H. (1989). Sensitive periods in
    development Structural characteristics and
    causal interpretations. Psychological Bulletin,
    105, 179-197.
  • Brovetto, C. (2002). The representation and
    processing of verbal morphology in the first and
    second language. Unpublished doctoral
    dissertation, Georgetown University, Washington,
    D.C.
  • Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P. W.,
    Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in
    infants. Science, 171, 303-306.
  • Eubank, L., Gregg, K. R. (1999). Critical
    periods and (second) language acquisition
    Divide et impera. In D. Birdsong (Ed.), Second
    language acquisition and the critical period
    hypothesis (pp. 65-99). Mahwah, NJ Erlbaum.
  • Flege, J. E. (1999). Age of learning and
    second-language speech. In D. Birdsong (Ed.),
    Second language acquisition and the critical
    period hypothesis (pp. 101-131). Mahwah, NJ
    Erlbaum.

47
References
  • Flege, J. E., Munro, J. J., MacKay, I. R. A.
    (1995). Factors affecting strength of perceived
    foreign accent in a second language. Journal of
    the Acoustical Society of America, 97, 3125-3134.
  • Flege, J. E., Yeni-Komshian, G. H., Liu, S.
    (1999). Age constraints on second-language
    acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 41,
    78-104.
  • Flynn, S., Manuel, S. (1991). Age-dependent
    effects in language acquisitoin Critical
    period hypotheses. In L. Eubank (Ed.),
    Point-counterpoint Universal grammar in the
    second language (pp. 117-145). Amsterdam
    Benjamins.
  • Grosjean, F. (1989). Studying bilinguals
    Methodological and conceptual issues.
    Bilingualism Language and Cognition, 1,
    131-149.
  • Hakuta, K., Bialystok, E., Wiley, E. (2001).
    Critical evidence A test of the critical period
    hypothesis for second language acquisition.
    Unpublished manuscript, Stanford University,
    California.

48
References
  • Hubel, D. H., Wiesel, T. N. (1965). Binocular
    interaction in the striate cortex of kittens
    reared with artificial squint. Journal of
    Neurophysiology, 21, 1041 1059.
  • Johnson, J. (1992). Critical period effects in
    second language acquisition The effect of
    written versus auditory materials on the
    assessment of grammatical competence. Language
    Learning, 42, 217-248.
  • Johnson, J., Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical
    period effects in second language learning The
    influence of maturational state on the
    acquisitoin of English as a second language.
    Cognitive Psychology, 21, 60-99.
  • Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological foundations
    of language. New York Wiley.
  • Long, M. H. (1990). Maturational constraints on
    language development. Studies in Second Language
    Acquisition, 12, 251-285.
  • Marchman, V. A. (1993). Constraints on
    plasticity in a connectionist model of the
    English past tense. Journal of Cognitive
    Neuroscience, 5, 215-234.

49
References
  • Marler, P. (1991). The instinct to learn. In S.
    Carey R. Gelman (Eds.), The epigenesis of mind
    Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 37-66).
    Hillsdale, NJ Erlbaum.
  • Moyer, A. (1999). Ultimate attainment in L2
    phonology. Studies in Second Language
    Acquisition, 21, 81-108.
  • Newport, E. L. (1991). Constrasting conceptions
    of the critical period for language. In S. Carey
    R. Gelman (Eds.), The epigenesis of mind
    Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 111-130).
    Hillsdale, NJ Erlbaum.
  • Patkowski, M. S. (1980). The sensitive period for
    the acquisition of syntax in a second language.
    Language Learning, 30, 449-472.
  • Patkowski, M. S. (1990). Age and accent in a
    second language A reply to James Emil Flege.
    Applied Linguistics, 11, 73-89.
  • Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct How
    the mind creates language. New York Morrow.

50
References
  • Pulvermüller, F. Schumann, J. H. (1994).
    Neurobiological mechanisms of language
    acquisition. Language Learning, 44, 681-734.
  • Scovel, T. (1988). A time to speak A
    psycholinguistic inquiry into the critical period
    for human speech. Rowley, MA Newbury House.
  • Seliger, H. W. (1978). Implications of a multiple
    critical periods hypothesis for second language
    learning. In W. Ritchie (Ed.), Second language
    acquisition research Issues and implications
    (pp. 11-19). New York Academic Press.
  • Selinker, L. (1972). Interlanguage.
    International Review of Applied Linguistics, 10,
    290-231.
  • Yeni-Komshian, G., Flege, J. E., Liu, S.
    (1997). Pronunciation proficiency in L1 and L2
    among Korean-English bilinguals The effect of
    age of arrival in the U.S. Journal of the
    Acoustical Society of America, 102(A), 3138.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com