Ten to Twelve Months - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

About This Presentation
Title:

Ten to Twelve Months

Description:

Ten to Twelve Months Fogel Chapter 8 Created by Ilse DeKoeyer-Laros, Ph.D. Experiential Exercises: Social Referencing If you are in the role of parent, you may also ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:413
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 170
Provided by: Goo7294
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Ten to Twelve Months


1
Ten to Twelve Months
  • Fogel
  • Chapter 8

Created by Ilse DeKoeyer-Laros, Ph.D.
2
Ten to Twelve Months
  • Physical and Motor Development
  • Perceptual Development
  • Cognitive Development
  • Emotional Development
  • Social and Language Development
  • Family and Society
  • Experiential Exercises

3
Ten to Twelve Months
  • From 10 to 12 months brings several of the most
    important achievements of the infancy period
  • infants develop strong and permanent attachments
    to their primary caregivers, typically the
    members of the immediate family
  • they prefer to stay close to these people, they
    do not like separations, and they may become
    afraid of strangers under certain circumstances
  • the infants sense of security or insecurity in
    those attachment ties has lasting implications
    for mental health and the success of
    interpersonal relationships
  • Infants develop a subjective sense of self
  • they learn ways to communicate their perceptions
    and feelings to others and at the same time learn
    that others have a view of the world different
    from their own

4
Physical Motor Development
  • By the age of 1 year, infant growth rates have
    leveled off
  • most year-old boys are between 28 1/4 inches
    (71.75 cm) and 32 inches (81.25 cm) in length
  • girls are between 27 1/2 inches (69.85 cm) and 31
    1/4 inches (79.38 cm)
  • both boys and girls will continue to grow at a
    rate of about 4 to 5 inches (10 to 13 cm) per
    year for the next several years
  • At 1 year, boys weigh between 18 1/2 and 26 1/2
    pounds (8.4 to 12.0 kg)
  • girls weigh between 17 1/4 and 24 3/4 pounds
    (7.8 to 11.2 kg)
  • By the 12th month, most babies are eating a
    variety of solid foods, including table foods
    that are cut up for them
  • most infants are holding their own spoons and can
    drink from a cup with both hands
  • by this time, almost all infants will have
    experienced teething pains and will have several
    teeth to help them chew their food

5
Physical Motor Development
  • 11 months is the average age at which infants can
    stand alone (the range is 9 to 16 months)
  • At 11 3/4 months, the average baby can walk alone
    (the range is 9 to 17 months)
  • By 1 year, most infants can sit down from a
    standing position, and most can climb up and down
    stairs by crawling

6
Physical Motor Development
  • Infants between 3 and 15 months were observed
    every 3 weeks in a laboratory going up and down a
    moderate slope
  • The methods used to go up and down depended upon
    whether the infant was a belly crawler (also
    called a creeper see Chapter 7), a
    hands-and-knees crawler, or a walker
  • Most of the infants adapted their locomotion by
    checking out the slope in relation to their
    abilities
  • The more experience they had with the slope, the
    more conservative they became in their method and
    the less need there was to rescue the infant from
    a possible fall
  • With experience, infants began to make smart
    locomotor decisions that required less adult
    guidance, and parents reported a similar
    progression at home

7
Physical Motor Development
  • Infants used four different strategies, depending
    upon their ability
  • there was the quick glance followed by a plunge,
    typically when infants felt the surface was safe
    enough to proceed
  • if the glance suggested some difficulty, the
    infants took a long look while swaying their
    bodies, then proceeded with their typical method
    of locomotion
  • if that did not work, infants took more time to
    look, and they also touched the slope to check
    the slant
  • this was sometimes followed by their typical
    means of locomotion and sometimes by an
    alternative means
  • if these techniques did not work, as a last
    resort, infants would hold onto the landing and
    try out different means of locomotion before
    proceeding
  • at this point, if they perceived the slope as too
    risky they would await a rescue from the adult
  • if not, they typically chose a sliding method

8
Physical Motor Development
  • The first months of independent walking initiate
    what some refer to as the toddler period of
    infant development
  • the label toddler seems to derive from the
    characteristic gait of the child who has not
    fully mastered the skill of walking
  • the earliest forms of childhood bipedalism have a
    distinct resemblance to a duck out for a jog
  • When adults walk, both legs are moving at the
    same time while one is moving forward, the other
    moves backward relative to the body
  • This kind of movement is called symmetrical gait
  • In the gait of toddlers, many steps are
    symmetrical, but many are also unsymmetrical
  • this happens because the toddlers often plant one
    foot and then seem to fall forward onto the other
    foot in a robot-like walk
  • toddlers still have trouble balancing and they
    need to walk this way to keep from falling

9
Physical Motor Development
  • Smoothness of gait reaches nearly adult levels
    about 6 months after the infant begins walking,
    regardless of the age at which the infant started
    taking steps
  • If infants are given support by an adult, their
    variability lessens, and they seem to be
    better-coordinated walkers
  • this suggests that balance, not the timing of the
    limb movements, is the limiting factor
  • balance improves gradually over time with
    increases in muscle strength and experience
    walking
  • Locomotion has benefits other than the ability to
    move from place to place
  • after beginning to walk, 10-month-old infants
    increase their frequency and duration of social
    contacts
  • this occurs whether the walking is supported or
    unsupported
  • it appears that an upright infant is more likely
    to be able to look, vocalize, and smile at adults

10
Physical Motor Development
  • Locomotor experience enhances cognitive
    development
  • one of the tests typically used to assess
    cognitive knowledge is the ability to search for
    hidden objects
  • infants with more locomotor experience, who are
    apparently more accustomed to moving around in
    the environment, are the most likely to persist
    in searches for hidden objects
  • The development of walking also facilitates the
    search for hidden objects by blind infants, even
    though blind infants walk later than sighted
    infants
  • Being able to move oneself through the
    environment is essential for understanding
    spatial arrangements and the locations of things
    in that space

11
Physical Motor Development
  • The motor skill of reaching continues to improve
    during this period
  • between 6 and 8 months, infants discover that
    they can lean forward to get objects just outside
    their reach
  • by 10 months, infants understand the limits of
    how far they can reach both by leaning forward
    and by extending their arms
  • by 12 months, infants are able to use mechanical
    and social aids to get things out of their reach,
    such as using a long object to get another or
    asking an adult for help

12
Physical Motor Development
  • Fine motor development is linked to cognitive and
    perceptual development
  • In one research study, infants at 6, 9, and 12
    months were observed while playing with objects
    that differed in weight, shape, or texture
  • infants were handed one object to explore and
    then given another differing only in one property
  • between 6 and 12 months, mouthing the objects
    decreased, while fingering increased.
  • the 12-month-olds also used more actions that
    were specific to the properties of the object
  • Related research has shown that by 12 months,
    infants use touching, listening, watching, and
    mouthing as alternative sources of information
    gathering
  • This involves more than intersensory
    coordination it is the coordination of different
    types of motor skills in the service of directed
    exploration of objects

13
Physical Motor Development
  • This research shows that by 12 months, infants
    are using specific actions that are adapted to
    the type of object they are holding
  • Motor skill improvements and improvements in the
    ability to relate information cross-modally (such
    as between vision and touch) are essential in
    fostering cognitive development because they put
    infants into direct contact with more aspects of
    their world

14
Perceptual Cognitive Development
Conceptualizing Relationships between Objects
Events
  • By the age of 7 months, infants are just
    beginning to understand that objects exist as
    whole entities and they are starting to
    categorize objects on the basis of their
    similarity to a prototype
  • By 10 months, infants are beginning to discover
    the relationships between objects, between people
    and objects, and between people
  • This can be seen in the development of relational
    play
  • relational play is action that demonstrates a
    knowledge of the relationships between two
    objects, for example, putting lids on pots, cups
    on saucers, or spoons in cups
  • the more perceptually distinct the two objects,
    the more likely it is that babies will combine
    them correctly

15
Perceptual Cognitive Development
Conceptualizing Relationships between Objects
Events
  • Infants of this age are also able to perceive the
    relationship between a tool and its use
  • Infants first saw a toy sitting on the far end of
    a long piece of cloth
  • The near end of the cloth was pulled by an
    experimenter, moving the toy closer to the
    experimenter
  • Next they saw the toy sitting next to the cloth
  • In one instance the cloth was pulled and the toy
    did not move closer (as expected) and in another
    instance the cloth was pulled and the toy moved
    closer (an impossible event)
  • The infants were surprised when seeing the
    impossible event, suggesting that they understood
    the relationship between the cloth tool and the
    object being retrieved

16
Perceptual Cognitive Development
Conceptualizing Relationships between Objects
Events
  • In another variant of this procedure, infants
    watched one puppet (the giver) give a flower to
    another puppet (the receiver)
  • when the two puppets positions were reversed,
    the infants still expected the original giver
    puppet to continue being the giver regardless of
    physical location
  • Forming relationships between objects can also be
    seen in studies in which infants were placed in
    front of a tray containing different groups of
    identical objects
  • for example, four identical human figures, four
    balls of the same color, and four identical toy
    cars
  • six-month-olds pick up the objects in random
    sequence, even though they can visually
    distinguish the different types of objects in a
    standard habituation procedure
  • by 12 months, infants will pick up three or four
    identical objects in a row before going on to
    pick up other objects

17
Perceptual Cognitive Development
Conceptualizing Relationships between Objects
Events
  • By 10 months, infants are able to classify
    pictures of animals (dogs versus cats), male
    versus female faces, and plants versus kitchen
    utensils
  • the ability of infants of this age to categorize
    objects is related to their familiarity with
    those objects rather than to some abstract
    ability to categorize objects
  • Infants of this age are also able to perceive the
    relationship between a prior event and a
    subsequent event, that is, between a cause and an
    effect
  • infants can understand that when one toy car hits
    another toy car, the second one should move as a
    result of the collision
  • they also understand the relationships between
    faces and voices, that a male voice belongs with
    a male face and a female voice belongs with a
    female face

18
Perceptual Cognitive Development
The Emergence of Infant Intentional Action
  • In these studies, infants are acting as if their
    object play has particular goals, such as
    combining objects in a meaningful way
  • This deliberate combination of different actions
    into a unified pattern of behavior suggests that
    infants are intending to act in this way
  • This is different from what occurred in earlier
    periods, when infants first discovered actions by
    chance and then repeated those actions as primary
    or secondary circular reactions
  • This intentional and deliberate form of action is
    what Piaget called the coordination of secondary
    circular reactions
  • In Observations 8.1 and 8.2, Jacquelines pushing
    away of her parents hand shows how the infant
    can combine different actions with each hand to
    achieve a goal

19
Perceptual Cognitive Development
The Emergence of Infant Intentional Action
  • Notice that to perform these deliberate actions,
    the infant must relate two simpler secondary
    circular reactions, such as holding the toy in
    one hand and pushing the adult away with the
    other
  • Infants are also relating two actions when they
    search for hidden objects
  • in order to find an object that they see being
    hidden behind a barrier or a cover, infants have
    to move the barrier with one hand and grasp the
    uncovered object with the other hand
  • Piaget found that by 10 months, infants will
    readily search for the hidden object and seem
    delighted to find it under the cover

20
Perceptual Cognitive Development
The Emergence of Infant Intentional Action
  • Now suppose you have two identical
    handkerchief-sized pieces of colored cloth on a
    table at which you are sitting opposite the
    infant
  • You engage the infant with an attractive toy,
    such as a set of colored keys, and then you hide
    the keys under one of the pieces of cloth
  • Infants 10 months and older, but not younger,
    will lift the cloth to retrieve the keys
  • Now suppose you take the keys back from the baby
    and hide them under the other piece of cloth
  • Infants younger than about 15 months typically
    will look under the first piece of cloth and will
    not persist in looking under the second piece of
    cloth to find the object even though it was
    hidden in their plain view

21
Perceptual Cognitive Development
The Emergence of Infant Intentional Action
  • This mistaken search for the missing object is
    called the A-not-B error by infancy researchers
  • the infants who find the object at location A,
    the first location, cannot find the object at
    location B, the second location
  • it seems as if infants lose the intention to find
    the object after more than one hiding
  • According to Piaget, the infants act as if part
    of their definition of the object includes its
    location
  • infants do not yet conceive of a whole
    independent object
  • infants define the object as the
    keys-under-the-cloth, or the ball-under-the-cha
    ir

22
Perceptual Cognitive Development
The Emergence of Infant Intentional Action
  • Following Piagets elegant first experiments and
    his contextual explanation, there have been a
    number of seemingly contradictory results
  • first of all, it should be noted that by 9
    months, infants are almost 100 correct in
    reaching for hidden objects at the A location
  • second, if objects are displaced at several
    different locations without being hidden or if
    objects are hidden under transparent covers, so
    long as the infants are first familiarized with
    the covers and objects, they are almost 100
    correct in reaching for the object in either
    location, A or B
  • the only case in which this is not true is if the
    object or the infant is moved along complex paths
    with many twists and turns
  • this suggests that following the path of a moving
    object in space is not limiting the infants
    search when the objects are hidden

23
Perceptual Cognitive Development
The Emergence of Infant Intentional Action
  • It appears, then, that infants of this age
    already have a concept of objects as existing
    when out of sight, and they do not appear to
    associate objects with particular locations,
    since they will directly search for the object in
    multiple locations so long as they can see the
    object

24
Perceptual Cognitive Development
The Emergence of Infant Intentional Action
  • The A-not-B error is made most frequently when
    the object is out of sight, but even then,
    infants succeed under certain conditions
  • 1. If there is no delay between the hiding and
    the opportunity to search for the object, they
    can find it
  • Errors are increased if infants are restrained
    for at least 3 seconds after the object is hidden
    at location B
  • 2. If the infants are shown the object being
    hidden in the A location multiple times, they are
    more likely to search in the B location
  • 3. If the infants are allowed to lean their
    bodies in the direction of the hidden object,
    they can sometimes find it even after a delay by
    following the direction of their lean
  • 4. If the objects are hidden under covers that
    are perceptually very different, it is easier for
    the infants than if the objects are hidden under
    identical covers
  • 5. It also helps if the infants are familiar with
    the objects or if the objects are interesting to
    them

25
Perceptual Cognitive Development
The Emergence of Infant Intentional Action
  • The A-not-B error is not a serious deficit for
    infants, and they overcome it within a few months
    or if the objects are presented in ways that
    facilitate their search
  • The importance of the A-not-B error lies in
    questions it raises for understanding human
    development
  • Development offers many examples in which the
    emergence of a new skill (like searching for
    hidden objects) is accompanied by a curious but
    not serious deficit (like being unable to search
    in more than one hiding place)

26
Perceptual Cognitive Development
Imitation
  • Imitation is a skill that requires making a
    conceptual relationship between two actions, in
    this case, between another persons actions and
    ones own
  • the imitations of newborns occur only for acts
    that they can already do
  • observing an adult doing the same types of acts
    increases the probability of the newborns
    selecting a similar act
  • newborn imitation is slow and does not happen for
    all infants
  • six-month-olds can imitate actions that they have
    not done before, but only if you give them many
    demonstrations and allow them plenty of time to
    process the information
  • between 10 and 12 months, infants become more
    proficient at imitating actions that they see for
    the first time or have not done before

27
Perceptual Cognitive Development
Imitation
  • Babies are better at imitating actions that are
    close to what they can already do
  • nine-month-old infants can imitate simple actions
    on objects such as opening a box, shaking a toy
    rattle, and pushing a button
  • in addition, when the same babies were shown the
    objects 24 hours later, they reproduced the
    actions that had been modeled previously
  • Imitation that occurs following a delay from the
    time the action is observed is called deferred
    imitation
  • deferred imitation also shows that infants can
    remember the relationships they learn, at least
    for a short time
  • if infants are allowed to imitate the action
    immediately, they can remember and imitate after
    longer delays compared to infants who were only
    allowed to watch and not imitate the action

28
Perceptual Cognitive Development
Individual Differences in Cognition Attention
  • The basic trends in cognitive development have
    been found to occur at about the same ages in
    different cultures around the world
  • within any group there are individual differences
    in the age of attainment of cognitive milestones
    and in the quality of cognitive abilities
  • One important component of cognition is the
    ability to attend to objects for a long enough
    time to remember their locations, watch their
    paths of movement, or learn about their
    properties during exploratory play
  • individual differences in the duration of
    sustained attention to objects have been found at
    the age of 1 year
  • infants who can sustain attention for longer
    periods engage in higher levels of exploratory
    play and score higher on developmental tests of
    mental and motor abilities

29
Perceptual Cognitive Development
Individual Differences in Cognition Attention
  • What factors account for individual differences
    in attention
  • to some extent, these differences may be related
    to differential development of the brain and may
    be partly constitutional
  • on the other hand, differences in the caregiving
    environment have been shown to influence the
    quality of infant attention
  • When mothers are trained to enhance their
    object-related behaviors during social play with
    infantsby demonstrating object properties,
    pointing to and naming objects, and
    questioningthe complexity of the infants
    exploratory play is enhanced
  • this effect works best for infants who have short
    attention spans
  • their duration of attention increases following
    an intervention in which adults work to point out
    object properties and refocus the infants after a
    loss of attention, while the duration of
    attention for high-attending infants does not
    change following the intervention

30
Perceptual Cognitive Development
Individual Differences in Cognition Attention
  • Another approach to the study of individual
    differences in cognitive ability is to assess the
    infants mastery motivation
  • mastery motivation is an inherent motivation to
    be competent in a particular situation, and its
    measurement involves persistence in solving
    problems
  • At 12 months of age, persistent goal-directed
    actions on objects are typically followed by the
    expression of smiling or laughter by infants,
    suggesting that persistence is in fact motivated
    by a goal and that the achievement of the goal
    results in positive feelings of efficacy

31
Perceptual Cognitive Development
Individual Differences in Cognition Attention
  • Adult object-related behavior increased the level
    of mastery motivation only for 12-month-olds who
    were rated as being temperamentally low in
    activity
  • For infants who were highly active, parental
    intervention had no effect or an interfering
    effect on the infants mastery
  • More active infants are less in need of adult
    encouragement and intervention in their play than
    less active infant

32
Perceptual Cognitive Development
Individual Differences in Cognition Attention
  • These studies suggest that adults can play
    important roles in the cognitive development of
    infants, particularly if their actions are
    designed to enhance the infants attention to
    objects and their properties
  • the adults need not so much teach or reinforce as
    support the infants own initiatives and help the
    infants to regulate their limited attention spans
  • a similar pattern of parental support also
    enhances word and language learning

33
Perceptual Cognitive Development
Individual Differences in Cognition Attention
  • These results also suggest that adult behavior
    needs to be adapted to the individual infant
  • babies who are less active and poor attenders may
    need a more involved adult to help organize their
    play
  • babies who are more active and attentive may have
    different needs for adults, perhaps needing them
    to be an appreciative audience to whom the
    infants can show off their self-directed
    achievements

34
Perceptual Cognitive Development
Individual Differences in Cognition Attention
  • Infants of this age are making the discovery that
    objects and events are related to each other
  • they can combine objects according to their
    function (lids on pots), their category (trucks
    vs. cars), or how they need to be combined to
    achieve a goal (using a fork to get food)
  • This ability to form mental relationships between
    things is related to important developments in
    emotion, communication, and the sense of self

35
Emotional Development
  • The infants life is becoming increasingly
    integrated into patterns of intentions and
    relationships
  • Motor skills create the tools with which the
    infant can operate on the environment to achieve
    goals, and goals increasingly structure the way
    in which the infant behaves
  • During the previous age period, infants became
    upset when someone caused them pain or when some
    expected event did not happen
  • between the ages of 10 and 12 months, infants
    become upset when their goals are blocked and are
    pleased when they achieve an intended goal
  • they also develop new emotions regarding their
    relationships with other people

36
Emotional Development
The Development of Anger
  • Anger is the emotion most frequently elicited in
    infants when their goals have been disrupted
  • An angry expression has distinct characteristics
  • the mouth is open with a squarish shape that is
    angled downward toward the back of the mouth
  • the brows are lowered, and the eyes are opened
    and intense
  • anger typically involves a bracing of the jaw
  • In the expression of distress, the mouth is
    similar, but the eyes are usually closed or
    partially closed

37
Emotional Development
The Development of Anger
  • When the infant is crying and making the anger
    expression at the same time, the state of emotion
    is more intense
  • When anger expressions were observed without
    crying, EEG recordings from the infants scalps
    showed heightened activity in the left frontal
    region
  • when the infants were angry and crying, there was
    more activity in the right frontal region
  • the right frontal area is believed to be
    associated with more-intense states of negative
    emotions
  • these results suggest that low levels of anger,
    without crying, are probably more
    cognitively-based (left brain) and maintain the
    infants orientation toward the environment

38
Emotional Development
The Development of Anger
  • The emotion of anger began in the previous stage
    as a more vigilant and intense form of distress
  • Between 10 and 12 months, anger becomes more
    purposeful and directed
  • infants do such things as stomp their feet, hit
    away objects or interfering hands, or slap and
    kick
  • these expressions have the quality of outbursts,
    and they coincide with the development of
    goal-directed behavior seen in other realms of
    infant functioning during this period

39
Emotional Development
The Development of Wariness Fear
  • Around the age of 6 months, infants develop a
    wary look, which may involve a raised brow
    furrowing above the nose and a relatively relaxed
    mouth
  • Wariness is related to the emotion of fear, since
    both involve an inhibition of action and may
    reflect a tendency for the individual to withdraw
    from the situation

40
Emotional Development
The Development of Wariness Fear
  • The expression of fear includes the raised and
    furrowed brow of wariness while the mouth corners
    are retracted straight back
  • true fear expressions are rare in infancy, but
    they first appear around the age of 10 months
  • fear expressions may appear briefly and then
    change to anger or sadness
  • fear is more likely to be expressed by behavioral
    inhibition in the absence of a facial expression
  • infants may stop their movements or actively
    avoid approaching the source of the fear. Infants
    feel fear when unexpected or threatening events
    occur

41
Emotional Development
The Development of Wariness Fear
  • Situations that may arouse fear in year-old
    infants
  • Heights
  • fear of heights has been assessed using the
    visual cliff situation
  • a piece of hard, clear plastic is extended over a
    box with a shallow side and a deep side
  • beginning at 9 months of age, infants show fear
    in approaching the deep side of the cliff
  • Unpredictable objects and movements
  • infants will show fear responses to any
    objectseither people or inanimate moving
    objectsthat loom unexpectedly in front of them
  • surprising events, like a jack-in-the-box popping
    up, may also cause fear
  • unpredictable, noisy mechanical toys can cause
    fear
  • however, in one study, infants were given control
    over the movements of such toys.
  • when the infants were in control, the toys were
    significantly less fearsome

42
Emotional Development
The Development of Wariness Fear
  • Acquired fears
  • infants may become fearful of an otherwise benign
    situation because it reminds them of something
    they found stressful, fearful, or painful in the
    past
  • these fears can be said to arise from a
    conditioned association
  • they are different from fears of such things as
    heights or looming objects, which may be
    universal
  • acquired fears are learned
  • examples are fear of particular people, of
    doctors offices, or of certain kinds of sounds,
    such as a dogs bark

43
Emotional Development
The Development of Wariness Fear
  • Strangers
  • Fear of strangers takes two forms
  • one is an acquired fear of particular people or
    people wearing a particular kind of clothing or
    hairstyle
  • the other is a general wariness of the unfamiliar
    that appears in most infants in every culture
    beginning about 8 months of age
  • Infants show less fear
  • if the stranger approaches them slowly and keeps
    an appropriate distance
  • if their mothers are present when the stranger
    approaches
  • if they are with familiar caregivers, such as
    baby-sitters or child-care providers
  • if the stranger is a little person or a child
  • if the stranger does not tower over them
  • if the stranger is sensitive to the infants
    signals and allows the approach to be regulated
    by the infants
  • if the infants are in an unfamiliar setting, such
    as a laboratory, compared to a home

44
Emotional Development
The Development of Wariness Fear
  • You might expect a baby to be less fearful at
    home than in a strange place, but this is not the
    case
  • when the stranger intrudes on the familiar and
    predictable setting of the home the infant gets
    disturbed
  • In strange places, infants seem to expect to see
    unusual or unfamiliar things
  • A number of studies have shown that babies can
    engage in positive and rewarding social
    interaction soon after meeting a new person
  • if the stranger proves acceptable to the baby,
    the baby often will spend more time playing with
    this interesting visitor than with his or her own
    mother

45
Emotional Development
The Development of Sadness
  • The emotion of sadness has a different expression
    than anger and fear
  • during sadness, the brows are raised at the
    center and drop at the sides, and the mouth
    corners are drawn back and down
  • Sadness without crying is less intense, showing
    left-frontal brain activity
  • with crying, sadness is accompanied by
    right-brain activation
  • in the earlier months, sadness accompanies
    disappointment when an expected event fails to
    happen
  • By 9 or 10 months, sadness accompanies a feeling
    of loss
  • because infants can now connect their memory of
    absent objects with some concrete action on the
    objects, the infants may become sad if an object
    disappears and they cannot find it after a search

46
Emotional Development
The Development of Sadness
  • In some cases, but not all, sadness accompanies
    separation from caregivers
  • this emotion is sometimes called separation
    distress
  • Research has shown that if mothers leave their
    babies behind in the company of the regular
    caregiver (the grandmother, baby-sitter, father),
    there is little or no separation distress
  • Infants respond more positively to separation
    from their mothers
  • if they are left with any other person,
    particularly a familiar one
  • if they are left with toys of any kind
  • if they can see or hear their mothers in an
    adjoining room
  • if they are left with their own pacifiers

47
Emotional Development
The Development of Sadness
  • The mothers' saying bye-bye or making some
    other parting gestures before they left had no
    effect on 1-year-olds
  • these parting gestures do seem to help older
    infants
  • the longer parents take to say good-bye, the
    harder it is for the babies to initially adjust
    to the new situation

48
Emotional Development
The Development of Sadness
  • There are cultural differences in infant response
    to separation, as shown by a study of brief
    (30-second) mother-infant separations in Japan
  • about half of the Japanese infants showed
    distress, even at this brief separation, perhaps
    because of the close contact between Japanese
    mothers and their infants and because separation
    is relatively rare
  • about one-third of the mothers apologized to the
    infant during the reunion
  • their apologies were done in an intonation
    pattern that matched the infants crying, such as
    saying Hai, hai, gomen nei, gomen nei, oh, oh
    (yes, yes, oh, Im so sorry, so sorry, well,
    well)
  • the mothers seemed to endorse the infants
    feelings as if to join in their misery and seek
    their forgiveness, a pattern of emotional sharing
    commonly seen in Japanese adults

49
Emotional Development
The Development of Sadness
  • If infants are separated from parents for long
    periods and are not provided with adequate
    substitute caregivers, more serious depression
    and withdrawal can result, including both
    behavior and physiological changes
  • These effects can be ameliorated to some extent
    once the infants are restored to stable adult
    care, either with their biological parents or
    with adoptive parents

50
Emotional Development
The Development of Enjoyment Affection
  • In the earlier months, infants showed positive
    responses to their caregivers
  • The smile of recognition appears at 2 months, and
    laughter of enjoyment during social play appears
    at 5 months
  • At 10 months, the infant has a deeper and more
    lasting type of positive feeling that has been
    called affection
  • Affection has a characteristic expression that is
    similar to a simple smile in the mouth region
    accompanied by a widening of the eyes
  • Such smiles occur at the approach of familiar
    caregivers and are accompanied by right-brain
    activation
  • Infants smiles at strangers usually lack the
    wide-eye component such smiles activate the left
    side of the brain

51
Emotional Development
The Development of Enjoyment Affection
  • After a brief separation, infants often feel
    genuinely happy to see the caregiver
  • this is a positive emotion that goes beyond the
    particular situation and expresses a lasting bond
  • infants express these feelings not only to
    caregivers but also to favorite toys and to
    siblings
  • Infants of this age smile more in the presence of
    people, especially familiar people, and their
    favorite objects
  • Smiling is related partly to emotion and partly
    to communication with others
  • smiling is a reflection of a socially shared
    emotion

52
Emotional Development
The Development of Enjoyment Affection
  • Differential responsiveness to people on reunion
    is probably one of the earliest and most reliable
    ways of telling who the most important people in
    a babys life are
  • In a stressful situation, if both the mother and
    another caregiver are present, infants will
    approach the mother
  • Even when infants spend more hours of the day in
    the company of caregivers, as on an Israeli
    kibbutz, they show more positive responses when
    reunited with their mothers after a brief
    separation than when reunited with their
    metapelets, or caregivers
  • These findings suggest that babies have a growing
    awareness of the specialness of certain people
    and respond to them in ways that communicate the
    depth of their feelings.

53
Emotional Development
The Development of Mixed Emotions
  • In one study, a stranger presented dolls and
    teddy bears to infants of this age
  • the babies alternatively reached out toward the
    objects and pulled their arms back
  • Another study found evidence for mixed emotions
    by naturalistic observation in the infants homes
  • this study found that pure expressions of joy,
    anger, distress, excitement, or fear occur only
    about half the time
  • the other half of the time the infants face
    expresses more than one emotion

54
Emotional Development
The Development of Mixed Emotions
  • Enjoyment, for example, often is expressed with
    elements of excitement or surprise, as when a
    smile is combined with a jaw-drop, producing a
    wide-open, smiling mouth
  • This is called a play smile
  • play smiles in 12-month-olds occur during games
    involving some physical activity or touch
    combined with an element of excitement
  • These are games like tickle, chase, and tossing
    the baby in the air
  • play smiles are more likely to occur in physical
    games and thus appear more during father-infant
    play than in mother-infant play
  • Smiles have also been observed to occur with nose
    wrinkles, blinks, blows, and waves each
    combination reflects a slight variation in the
    meaning of the enjoyment

55
Emotional Development
The Development of Mixed Emotions
  • In an effort to regulate emotion, distress will
    sometimes be combined with biting or stiffening
    the lip, showing that the baby is trying to
    control the crying
  • one-year-old babies often alternate between
    distress and enjoyment, showing that they are
    able to maintain interest in a toy or in social
    interaction even though they may be tired or
    frustrated
  • One-year-olds will sometimes look directly at the
    caregiver when crying, or they may pull at the
    caregiver, gesture to be picked up, or point to
    the offending object or event
  • relaxed touching and holding by the caregiver can
    provide encouragement for the infants efforts to
    gain control over frustration, teaching the baby
    the capacity to persist
  • the baby can learn that feeling distressed and
    frustrated does not necessarily mean the end of
    the gamethat these negative affects are both
    tolerable and manageable

56
Emotional Development
The Development of Mixed Emotions
  • Another example of a mixed emotion appearing at
    this age is jealousy
  • Year-old infants were observed while their
    mothers and a strange adult female played with a
    picture book or a child-sized doll
  • infants showed more protest and negative
    vocalization and their play was more inhibited
    primarily in the mother-with-doll condition
  • these responses were interpreted as jealousy
    because the infant was not upset about the mother
    with a book, nor about the strangers behavior
  • jealousy is an example of wanting to approach the
    mother but avoiding her at the same time

57
Emotional Development
The Development of Mixed Emotions
  • Because of the emergence of mixed emotions, new
    forms of parent-child play develop as a way to
    help infants understand and cope with emotional
    changes
  • Teasing games are especially important in this
    regard
  • a tease takes something that has a serious
    emotional tone and alters it into a more positive
    tone
  • between 9 and 12 months, infants learn the art of
    teasing and also become able to appreciate the
    teases of others
  • the contradictory messages in these games provide
    important opportunities for infants to learn how
    to regulate their emotions, how to affect other
    people, and also how to have
  • teasing is relatively more frequent in
    father-infant than in mother-infant play

58
Emotional Development
The Development of Mixed Emotions
  • These findings show that infants are becoming
    emotionally complex
  • Not only are they aware of the relationships
    between objects and between events, they are also
    aware of the relationships between their
    different feelings

59
Emotional Development
Individual Differences in Emotion Regulation
  • Infants of this age are learning emotion
    regulation to maintain some self-control in the
    face of highly arousing (either exciting or
    distressing) situations
  • At this age, infants show differences in how they
    use people or objects to regulate emotions

60
Emotional Development
Individual Differences in Emotion Regulation
  • Heart-rate measures have shown that even when
    infants do not cry during maternal separation,
    they do get aroused
  • we may observe an extended glance at the door,
    perhaps a sad expression, and then a concerted
    effort to become involved with the toys
  • it is almost as if the babies were using the toys
    to prevent feeling sad and lonely
  • other infants who may ignore their mother when
    she returns from a separation also show elevated
    heart rates
  • these babies are coping with their ambivalent
    feelings about their mother in relation to the
    feeling of loss they must have experienced when
    she was out of the room
  • the manner in which the infants cope with these
    feelings is not necessarily adaptive, since it
    effectively removes them from the only source of
    comfort they might receive their mothers

61
Emotional Development
Individual Differences in Emotion Regulation
  • Some infants are temperamentally more fearful and
    withdrawn, and they are more likely to become
    fearful or sad in stressful situations, such as
    during separations and in the presence of
    strangers or unusual situations
  • These differences in emotionality are associated
    with individual differences in brain asymmetry
  • infants who become easily distressed to the point
    of crying by a maternal separation have more
    marked differences between left- and right-brain
    activity in the frontal area
  • these findings show that the infants ability to
    remain attentive to the situation, rather than to
    withdraw, may be in part responsible for higher
    levels of regulation of fear

62
Emotional Development
Individual Differences in Emotion Regulation
  • This suggests that there is a relationship
    between cognition (attention) and emotion
    regulation
  • some infants are more focused, attentive, and not
    easily upset
  • others seem more emotional and require more
    support from adults in challenging situations

63
Emotional Development
Developments in the Infants Ability to Perceive
Emotion Intention Expressed by Other People
  • By 6 or 7 months, infants are capable of
    perceiving a few simple facial expressions
  • By 9 or 10 months, infants can distinguish more
    expressions, and they are beginning to use the
    emotional information displayed by others in a
    meaningful manner
  • These developments in the perception of emotions
    in other people are one of the ways in which the
    infant is becoming increasingly aware of
    relationships

64
Emotional Development
Developments in the Infants Ability to Perceive
Emotion Intention Expressed by Other People
  • Affective sharing occurs when infants wish to
    communicate their feelings to another person or
    to confirm their feelings with another person
  • between 9 and 12 months of age, affective sharing
    is the most common response in social situations,
    and it tends almost always to involve positive
    emotions

65
Emotional Development
Developments in the Infants Ability to Perceive
Emotion Intention Expressed by Other People
  • Social referencing occurs when infants face an
    uncertain situation. In this case, they look to
    another persons emotional expressions to help
    them decide what to do in this situation
  • between 10 and 12 months, infants begin to use
    social referencing systematically, although
    social referencing does not occur as frequently
    as affective sharing in naturalistic situations
  • by 10 months, if mothers display a negative
    expression, some infants will avoid crossing a
    visual cliff, avoid playing with toys or avoid
    approaching a rabbit in a cage, and they will
    show more aversive responses to strangers to whom
    the mothers show negative expressions
  • negative expressions seem to have a more powerful
    effect on regulating infant behavior at this age
  • opposite patterns of behavior can be seen if the
    mothers display positive expressions in these
    situations

66
Emotional Development
Developments in the Infants Ability to Perceive
Emotion Intention Expressed by Other People
  • Underlying both affective sharing and social
    referencing is the development of the ability to
    delay an immediate emotional reaction to a
    situation and to evaluate how one is going to
    feel about the situation
  • Appraisal is the ability to use cognitive
    comparisons of alternate interpretations to
    regulate ones emotions
  • the appraisal at this age differs from the
    appraisal seen during the period between 6 and 9
    months
  • in that earlier period, infants are often
    observed to show a slight delay in their
    emotional reactions, as if deciding what to feel
    in a particular situation
  • after 10 months the infant looks to another
    person to decide what to feel

67
Emotional Development
Developments in the Infants Ability to Perceive
Emotion Intention Expressed by Other People
  • Fe
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com