Title: Ten to Twelve Months
1Ten to Twelve Months
Created by Ilse DeKoeyer-Laros, Ph.D.
2Ten to Twelve Months
- Physical and Motor Development
- Perceptual Development
- Cognitive Development
- Emotional Development
- Social and Language Development
- Family and Society
3Ten to Twelve Months
- From 10 to 12 months brings several of the most
important achievements of the infancy period - infants develop strong and permanent attachments
to their primary caregivers, typically the
members of the immediate family - they prefer to stay close to these people, they
do not like separations, and they may become
afraid of strangers under certain circumstances - the infants sense of security or insecurity in
those attachment ties has lasting implications
for mental health and the success of
interpersonal relationships - Infants develop a subjective sense of self
- they learn ways to communicate their perceptions
and feelings to others and at the same time learn
that others have a view of the world different
from their own
4Physical Motor Development
- By the age of 1 year, infant growth rates have
leveled off - most year-old boys are between 28 1/4 inches
(71.75 cm) and 32 inches (81.25 cm) in length - girls are between 27 1/2 inches (69.85 cm) and 31
1/4 inches (79.38 cm) - both boys and girls will continue to grow at a
rate of about 4 to 5 inches (10 to 13 cm) per
year for the next several years - At 1 year, boys weigh between 18 1/2 and 26 1/2
pounds (8.4 to 12.0 kg) - girls weigh between 17 1/4 and 24 3/4 pounds
(7.8 to 11.2 kg) - By the 12th month, most babies are eating a
variety of solid foods, including table foods
that are cut up for them - most infants are holding their own spoons and can
drink from a cup with both hands - by this time, almost all infants will have
experienced teething pains and will have several
teeth to help them chew their food
5Physical Motor Development
- 11 months is the average age at which infants can
stand alone (the range is 9 to 16 months) - At 11 3/4 months, the average baby can walk alone
(the range is 9 to 17 months) - By 1 year, most infants can sit down from a
standing position, and most can climb up and down
stairs by crawling
6Physical Motor Development
- Infants between 3 and 15 months were observed
every 3 weeks in a laboratory going up and down a
moderate slope - The methods used to go up and down depended upon
whether the infant was a belly crawler (also
called a creeper see Chapter 7), a
hands-and-knees crawler, or a walker - Most of the infants adapted their locomotion by
checking out the slope in relation to their
abilities - The more experience they had with the slope, the
more conservative they became in their method and
the less need there was to rescue the infant from
a possible fall - With experience, infants began to make smart
locomotor decisions that required less adult
guidance, and parents reported a similar
progression at home
7Physical Motor Development
- Infants used four different strategies, depending
upon their ability - there was the quick glance followed by a plunge,
typically when infants felt the surface was safe
enough to proceed - if the glance suggested some difficulty, the
infants took a long look while swaying their
bodies, then proceeded with their typical method
of locomotion - if that did not work, infants took more time to
look, and they also touched the slope to check
the slant - this was sometimes followed by their typical
means of locomotion and sometimes by an
alternative means - if these techniques did not work, as a last
resort, infants would hold onto the landing and
try out different means of locomotion before
proceeding - at this point, if they perceived the slope as too
risky they would await a rescue from the adult - if not, they typically chose a sliding method
8Physical Motor Development
- The first months of independent walking initiate
what some refer to as the toddler period of
infant development - the label toddler seems to derive from the
characteristic gait of the child who has not
fully mastered the skill of walking - the earliest forms of childhood bipedalism have a
distinct resemblance to a duck out for a jog - When adults walk, both legs are moving at the
same time while one is moving forward, the other
moves backward relative to the body - This kind of movement is called symmetrical gait
- In the gait of toddlers, many steps are
symmetrical, but many are also unsymmetrical - this happens because the toddlers often plant one
foot and then seem to fall forward onto the other
foot in a robot-like walk - toddlers still have trouble balancing and they
need to walk this way to keep from falling
9Physical Motor Development
- Smoothness of gait reaches nearly adult levels
about 6 months after the infant begins walking,
regardless of the age at which the infant started
taking steps - If infants are given support by an adult, their
variability lessens, and they seem to be
better-coordinated walkers - this suggests that balance, not the timing of the
limb movements, is the limiting factor - balance improves gradually over time with
increases in muscle strength and experience
walking - Locomotion has benefits other than the ability to
move from place to place - after beginning to walk, 10-month-old infants
increase their frequency and duration of social
contacts - this occurs whether the walking is supported or
unsupported - it appears that an upright infant is more likely
to be able to look, vocalize, and smile at adults
10Physical Motor Development
- Locomotor experience enhances cognitive
development - one of the tests typically used to assess
cognitive knowledge is the ability to search for
hidden objects - infants with more locomotor experience, who are
apparently more accustomed to moving around in
the environment, are the most likely to persist
in searches for hidden objects - The development of walking also facilitates the
search for hidden objects by blind infants, even
though blind infants walk later than sighted
infants - Being able to move oneself through the
environment is essential for understanding
spatial arrangements and the locations of things
in that space
11Physical Motor Development
- The motor skill of reaching continues to improve
during this period - between 6 and 8 months, infants discover that
they can lean forward to get objects just outside
their reach - by 10 months, infants understand the limits of
how far they can reach both by leaning forward
and by extending their arms - by 12 months, infants are able to use mechanical
and social aids to get things out of their reach,
such as using a long object to get another or
asking an adult for help
12Physical Motor Development
- Fine motor development is linked to cognitive and
perceptual development - In one research study, infants at 6, 9, and 12
months were observed while playing with objects
that differed in weight, shape, or texture - infants were handed one object to explore and
then given another differing only in one property
- between 6 and 12 months, mouthing the objects
decreased, while fingering increased. - the 12-month-olds also used more actions that
were specific to the properties of the object - Related research has shown that by 12 months,
infants use touching, listening, watching, and
mouthing as alternative sources of information
gathering - This involves more than intersensory
coordination it is the coordination of different
types of motor skills in the service of directed
exploration of objects
13Physical Motor Development
- This research shows that by 12 months, infants
are using specific actions that are adapted to
the type of object they are holding - Motor skill improvements and improvements in the
ability to relate information cross-modally (such
as between vision and touch) are essential in
fostering cognitive development because they put
infants into direct contact with more aspects of
their world
14Perceptual Cognitive Development
Conceptualizing Relationships between Objects
Events
- By the age of 7 months, infants are just
beginning to understand that objects exist as
whole entities and they are starting to
categorize objects on the basis of their
similarity to a prototype - By 10 months, infants are beginning to discover
the relationships between objects, between people
and objects, and between people - This can be seen in the development of relational
play - relational play is action that demonstrates a
knowledge of the relationships between two
objects, for example, putting lids on pots, cups
on saucers, or spoons in cups - the more perceptually distinct the two objects,
the more likely it is that babies will combine
them correctly
15Perceptual Cognitive Development
Conceptualizing Relationships between Objects
Events
- Infants of this age are also able to perceive the
relationship between a tool and its use - Infants first saw a toy sitting on the far end of
a long piece of cloth - The near end of the cloth was pulled by an
experimenter, moving the toy closer to the
experimenter - Next they saw the toy sitting next to the cloth
- In one instance the cloth was pulled and the toy
did not move closer (as expected) and in another
instance the cloth was pulled and the toy moved
closer (an impossible event) - The infants were surprised when seeing the
impossible event, suggesting that they understood
the relationship between the cloth tool and the
object being retrieved
16Perceptual Cognitive Development
Conceptualizing Relationships between Objects
Events
- In another variant of this procedure, infants
watched one puppet (the giver) give a flower to
another puppet (the receiver) - when the two puppets positions were reversed,
the infants still expected the original giver
puppet to continue being the giver regardless of
physical location - Forming relationships between objects can also be
seen in studies in which infants were placed in
front of a tray containing different groups of
identical objects - for example, four identical human figures, four
balls of the same color, and four identical toy
cars - six-month-olds pick up the objects in random
sequence, even though they can visually
distinguish the different types of objects in a
standard habituation procedure - by 12 months, infants will pick up three or four
identical objects in a row before going on to
pick up other objects
17Perceptual Cognitive Development
Conceptualizing Relationships between Objects
Events
- By 10 months, infants are able to classify
pictures of animals (dogs versus cats), male
versus female faces, and plants versus kitchen
utensils - the ability of infants of this age to categorize
objects is related to their familiarity with
those objects rather than to some abstract
ability to categorize objects - Infants of this age are also able to perceive the
relationship between a prior event and a
subsequent event, that is, between a cause and an
effect - infants can understand that when one toy car hits
another toy car, the second one should move as a
result of the collision - they also understand the relationships between
faces and voices, that a male voice belongs with
a male face and a female voice belongs with a
female face
18Perceptual Cognitive Development
The Emergence of Infant Intentional Action
- In these studies, infants are acting as if their
object play has particular goals, such as
combining objects in a meaningful way - This deliberate combination of different actions
into a unified pattern of behavior suggests that
infants are intending to act in this way - This is different from what occurred in earlier
periods, when infants first discovered actions by
chance and then repeated those actions as primary
or secondary circular reactions - This intentional and deliberate form of action is
what Piaget called the coordination of secondary
circular reactions - In Observations 8.1 and 8.2, Jacquelines pushing
away of her parents hand shows how the infant
can combine different actions with each hand to
achieve a goal
19Perceptual Cognitive Development
The Emergence of Infant Intentional Action
- Notice that to perform these deliberate actions,
the infant must relate two simpler secondary
circular reactions, such as holding the toy in
one hand and pushing the adult away with the
other - Infants are also relating two actions when they
search for hidden objects - in order to find an object that they see being
hidden behind a barrier or a cover, infants have
to move the barrier with one hand and grasp the
uncovered object with the other hand - Piaget found that by 10 months, infants will
readily search for the hidden object and seem
delighted to find it under the cover
20Perceptual Cognitive Development
The Emergence of Infant Intentional Action
- Now suppose you have two identical
handkerchief-sized pieces of colored cloth on a
table at which you are sitting opposite the
infant - You engage the infant with an attractive toy,
such as a set of colored keys, and then you hide
the keys under one of the pieces of cloth - Infants 10 months and older, but not younger,
will lift the cloth to retrieve the keys - Now suppose you take the keys back from the baby
and hide them under the other piece of cloth - Infants younger than about 15 months typically
will look under the first piece of cloth and will
not persist in looking under the second piece of
cloth to find the object even though it was
hidden in their plain view
21Perceptual Cognitive Development
The Emergence of Infant Intentional Action
- This mistaken search for the missing object is
called the A-not-B error by infancy researchers - the infants who find the object at location A,
the first location, cannot find the object at
location B, the second location - it seems as if infants lose the intention to find
the object after more than one hiding - According to Piaget, the infants act as if part
of their definition of the object includes its
location - infants do not yet conceive of a whole
independent object - infants define the object as the
keys-under-the-cloth, or the ball-under-the-cha
ir
22Perceptual Cognitive Development
The Emergence of Infant Intentional Action
- Following Piagets elegant first experiments and
his contextual explanation, there have been a
number of seemingly contradictory results - first of all, it should be noted that by 9
months, infants are almost 100 correct in
reaching for hidden objects at the A location - second, if objects are displaced at several
different locations without being hidden or if
objects are hidden under transparent covers, so
long as the infants are first familiarized with
the covers and objects, they are almost 100
correct in reaching for the object in either
location, A or B - the only case in which this is not true is if the
object or the infant is moved along complex paths
with many twists and turns - this suggests that following the path of a moving
object in space is not limiting the infants
search when the objects are hidden
23Perceptual Cognitive Development
The Emergence of Infant Intentional Action
- It appears, then, that infants of this age
already have a concept of objects as existing
when out of sight, and they do not appear to
associate objects with particular locations,
since they will directly search for the object in
multiple locations so long as they can see the
object
24Perceptual Cognitive Development
The Emergence of Infant Intentional Action
- The A-not-B error is made most frequently when
the object is out of sight, but even then,
infants succeed under certain conditions - 1. If there is no delay between the hiding and
the opportunity to search for the object, they
can find it - Errors are increased if infants are restrained
for at least 3 seconds after the object is hidden
at location B - 2. If the infants are shown the object being
hidden in the A location multiple times, they are
more likely to search in the B location - 3. If the infants are allowed to lean their
bodies in the direction of the hidden object,
they can sometimes find it even after a delay by
following the direction of their lean - 4. If the objects are hidden under covers that
are perceptually very different, it is easier for
the infants than if the objects are hidden under
identical covers - 5. It also helps if the infants are familiar with
the objects or if the objects are interesting to
them
25Perceptual Cognitive Development
The Emergence of Infant Intentional Action
- The A-not-B error is not a serious deficit for
infants, and they overcome it within a few months
or if the objects are presented in ways that
facilitate their search - The importance of the A-not-B error lies in
questions it raises for understanding human
development - Development offers many examples in which the
emergence of a new skill (like searching for
hidden objects) is accompanied by a curious but
not serious deficit (like being unable to search
in more than one hiding place)
26Perceptual Cognitive Development
Imitation
- Imitation is a skill that requires making a
conceptual relationship between two actions, in
this case, between another persons actions and
ones own - the imitations of newborns occur only for acts
that they can already do - observing an adult doing the same types of acts
increases the probability of the newborns
selecting a similar act - newborn imitation is slow and does not happen for
all infants - six-month-olds can imitate actions that they have
not done before, but only if you give them many
demonstrations and allow them plenty of time to
process the information - between 10 and 12 months, infants become more
proficient at imitating actions that they see for
the first time or have not done before
27Perceptual Cognitive Development
Imitation
- Babies are better at imitating actions that are
close to what they can already do - nine-month-old infants can imitate simple actions
on objects such as opening a box, shaking a toy
rattle, and pushing a button - in addition, when the same babies were shown the
objects 24 hours later, they reproduced the
actions that had been modeled previously - Imitation that occurs following a delay from the
time the action is observed is called deferred
imitation - deferred imitation also shows that infants can
remember the relationships they learn, at least
for a short time - if infants are allowed to imitate the action
immediately, they can remember and imitate after
longer delays compared to infants who were only
allowed to watch and not imitate the action
28Perceptual Cognitive Development
Individual Differences in Cognition Attention
- The basic trends in cognitive development have
been found to occur at about the same ages in
different cultures around the world - within any group there are individual differences
in the age of attainment of cognitive milestones
and in the quality of cognitive abilities - One important component of cognition is the
ability to attend to objects for a long enough
time to remember their locations, watch their
paths of movement, or learn about their
properties during exploratory play - individual differences in the duration of
sustained attention to objects have been found at
the age of 1 year - infants who can sustain attention for longer
periods engage in higher levels of exploratory
play and score higher on developmental tests of
mental and motor abilities
29Perceptual Cognitive Development
Individual Differences in Cognition Attention
- What factors account for individual differences
in attention - to some extent, these differences may be related
to differential development of the brain and may
be partly constitutional - on the other hand, differences in the caregiving
environment have been shown to influence the
quality of infant attention - When mothers are trained to enhance their
object-related behaviors during social play with
infantsby demonstrating object properties,
pointing to and naming objects, and
questioningthe complexity of the infants
exploratory play is enhanced - this effect works best for infants who have short
attention spans - their duration of attention increases following
an intervention in which adults work to point out
object properties and refocus the infants after a
loss of attention, while the duration of
attention for high-attending infants does not
change following the intervention
30Perceptual Cognitive Development
Individual Differences in Cognition Attention
- Another approach to the study of individual
differences in cognitive ability is to assess the
infants mastery motivation - mastery motivation is an inherent motivation to
be competent in a particular situation, and its
measurement involves persistence in solving
problems - At 12 months of age, persistent goal-directed
actions on objects are typically followed by the
expression of smiling or laughter by infants,
suggesting that persistence is in fact motivated
by a goal and that the achievement of the goal
results in positive feelings of efficacy
31Perceptual Cognitive Development
Individual Differences in Cognition Attention
- Adult object-related behavior increased the level
of mastery motivation only for 12-month-olds who
were rated as being temperamentally low in
activity - For infants who were highly active, parental
intervention had no effect or an interfering
effect on the infants mastery - More active infants are less in need of adult
encouragement and intervention in their play than
less active infant
32Perceptual Cognitive Development
Individual Differences in Cognition Attention
- These studies suggest that adults can play
important roles in the cognitive development of
infants, particularly if their actions are
designed to enhance the infants attention to
objects and their properties - the adults need not so much teach or reinforce as
support the infants own initiatives and help the
infants to regulate their limited attention spans
- a similar pattern of parental support also
enhances word and language learning
33Perceptual Cognitive Development
Individual Differences in Cognition Attention
- These results also suggest that adult behavior
needs to be adapted to the individual infant - babies who are less active and poor attenders may
need a more involved adult to help organize their
play - babies who are more active and attentive may have
different needs for adults, perhaps needing them
to be an appreciative audience to whom the
infants can show off their self-directed
achievements
34Perceptual Cognitive Development
Individual Differences in Cognition Attention
- Infants of this age are making the discovery that
objects and events are related to each other - they can combine objects according to their
function (lids on pots), their category (trucks
vs. cars), or how they need to be combined to
achieve a goal (using a fork to get food) - This ability to form mental relationships between
things is related to important developments in
emotion, communication, and the sense of self
35Emotional Development
- The infants life is becoming increasingly
integrated into patterns of intentions and
relationships - Motor skills create the tools with which the
infant can operate on the environment to achieve
goals, and goals increasingly structure the way
in which the infant behaves - During the previous age period, infants became
upset when someone caused them pain or when some
expected event did not happen - between the ages of 10 and 12 months, infants
become upset when their goals are blocked and are
pleased when they achieve an intended goal - they also develop new emotions regarding their
relationships with other people
36Emotional Development
The Development of Anger
- Anger is the emotion most frequently elicited in
infants when their goals have been disrupted - An angry expression has distinct characteristics
- the mouth is open with a squarish shape that is
angled downward toward the back of the mouth - the brows are lowered, and the eyes are opened
and intense - anger typically involves a bracing of the jaw
- In the expression of distress, the mouth is
similar, but the eyes are usually closed or
partially closed
37Emotional Development
The Development of Anger
- When the infant is crying and making the anger
expression at the same time, the state of emotion
is more intense - When anger expressions were observed without
crying, EEG recordings from the infants scalps
showed heightened activity in the left frontal
region - when the infants were angry and crying, there was
more activity in the right frontal region - the right frontal area is believed to be
associated with more-intense states of negative
emotions - these results suggest that low levels of anger,
without crying, are probably more
cognitively-based (left brain) and maintain the
infants orientation toward the environment
38Emotional Development
The Development of Anger
- The emotion of anger began in the previous stage
as a more vigilant and intense form of distress - Between 10 and 12 months, anger becomes more
purposeful and directed - infants do such things as stomp their feet, hit
away objects or interfering hands, or slap and
kick - these expressions have the quality of outbursts,
and they coincide with the development of
goal-directed behavior seen in other realms of
infant functioning during this period
39Emotional Development
The Development of Wariness Fear
- Around the age of 6 months, infants develop a
wary look, which may involve a raised brow
furrowing above the nose and a relatively relaxed
mouth - Wariness is related to the emotion of fear, since
both involve an inhibition of action and may
reflect a tendency for the individual to withdraw
from the situation
40Emotional Development
The Development of Wariness Fear
- The expression of fear includes the raised and
furrowed brow of wariness while the mouth corners
are retracted straight back - true fear expressions are rare in infancy, but
they first appear around the age of 10 months - fear expressions may appear briefly and then
change to anger or sadness - fear is more likely to be expressed by behavioral
inhibition in the absence of a facial expression - infants may stop their movements or actively
avoid approaching the source of the fear. Infants
feel fear when unexpected or threatening events
occur
41Emotional Development
The Development of Wariness Fear
- Situations that may arouse fear in year-old
infants - Heights
- fear of heights has been assessed using the
visual cliff situation - a piece of hard, clear plastic is extended over a
box with a shallow side and a deep side - beginning at 9 months of age, infants show fear
in approaching the deep side of the cliff - Unpredictable objects and movements
- infants will show fear responses to any
objectseither people or inanimate moving
objectsthat loom unexpectedly in front of them - surprising events, like a jack-in-the-box popping
up, may also cause fear - unpredictable, noisy mechanical toys can cause
fear - however, in one study, infants were given control
over the movements of such toys. - when the infants were in control, the toys were
significantly less fearsome
42Emotional Development
The Development of Wariness Fear
- Acquired fears
- infants may become fearful of an otherwise benign
situation because it reminds them of something
they found stressful, fearful, or painful in the
past - these fears can be said to arise from a
conditioned association - they are different from fears of such things as
heights or looming objects, which may be
universal - acquired fears are learned
- examples are fear of particular people, of
doctors offices, or of certain kinds of sounds,
such as a dogs bark
43Emotional Development
The Development of Wariness Fear
- Strangers
- Fear of strangers takes two forms
- one is an acquired fear of particular people or
people wearing a particular kind of clothing or
hairstyle - the other is a general wariness of the unfamiliar
that appears in most infants in every culture
beginning about 8 months of age - Infants show less fear
- if the stranger approaches them slowly and keeps
an appropriate distance - if their mothers are present when the stranger
approaches - if they are with familiar caregivers, such as
baby-sitters or child-care providers - if the stranger is a little person or a child
- if the stranger does not tower over them
- if the stranger is sensitive to the infants
signals and allows the approach to be regulated
by the infants - if the infants are in an unfamiliar setting, such
as a laboratory, compared to a home
44Emotional Development
The Development of Wariness Fear
- You might expect a baby to be less fearful at
home than in a strange place, but this is not the
case - when the stranger intrudes on the familiar and
predictable setting of the home the infant gets
disturbed - In strange places, infants seem to expect to see
unusual or unfamiliar things - A number of studies have shown that babies can
engage in positive and rewarding social
interaction soon after meeting a new person - if the stranger proves acceptable to the baby,
the baby often will spend more time playing with
this interesting visitor than with his or her own
mother
45Emotional Development
The Development of Sadness
- The emotion of sadness has a different expression
than anger and fear - during sadness, the brows are raised at the
center and drop at the sides, and the mouth
corners are drawn back and down - Sadness without crying is less intense, showing
left-frontal brain activity - with crying, sadness is accompanied by
right-brain activation - in the earlier months, sadness accompanies
disappointment when an expected event fails to
happen - By 9 or 10 months, sadness accompanies a feeling
of loss - because infants can now connect their memory of
absent objects with some concrete action on the
objects, the infants may become sad if an object
disappears and they cannot find it after a search
46Emotional Development
The Development of Sadness
- In some cases, but not all, sadness accompanies
separation from caregivers - this emotion is sometimes called separation
distress - Research has shown that if mothers leave their
babies behind in the company of the regular
caregiver (the grandmother, baby-sitter, father),
there is little or no separation distress - Infants respond more positively to separation
from their mothers - if they are left with any other person,
particularly a familiar one - if they are left with toys of any kind
- if they can see or hear their mothers in an
adjoining room - if they are left with their own pacifiers
47Emotional Development
The Development of Sadness
- The mothers' saying bye-bye or making some
other parting gestures before they left had no
effect on 1-year-olds - these parting gestures do seem to help older
infants - the longer parents take to say good-bye, the
harder it is for the babies to initially adjust
to the new situation
48Emotional Development
The Development of Sadness
- There are cultural differences in infant response
to separation, as shown by a study of brief
(30-second) mother-infant separations in Japan - about half of the Japanese infants showed
distress, even at this brief separation, perhaps
because of the close contact between Japanese
mothers and their infants and because separation
is relatively rare - about one-third of the mothers apologized to the
infant during the reunion - their apologies were done in an intonation
pattern that matched the infants crying, such as
saying Hai, hai, gomen nei, gomen nei, oh, oh
(yes, yes, oh, Im so sorry, so sorry, well,
well) - the mothers seemed to endorse the infants
feelings as if to join in their misery and seek
their forgiveness, a pattern of emotional sharing
commonly seen in Japanese adults
49Emotional Development
The Development of Sadness
- If infants are separated from parents for long
periods and are not provided with adequate
substitute caregivers, more serious depression
and withdrawal can result, including both
behavior and physiological changes - These effects can be ameliorated to some extent
once the infants are restored to stable adult
care, either with their biological parents or
with adoptive parents
50Emotional Development
The Development of Enjoyment Affection
- In the earlier months, infants showed positive
responses to their caregivers - The smile of recognition appears at 2 months, and
laughter of enjoyment during social play appears
at 5 months - At 10 months, the infant has a deeper and more
lasting type of positive feeling that has been
called affection - Affection has a characteristic expression that is
similar to a simple smile in the mouth region
accompanied by a widening of the eyes - Such smiles occur at the approach of familiar
caregivers and are accompanied by right-brain
activation - Infants smiles at strangers usually lack the
wide-eye component such smiles activate the left
side of the brain
51Emotional Development
The Development of Enjoyment Affection
- After a brief separation, infants often feel
genuinely happy to see the caregiver - this is a positive emotion that goes beyond the
particular situation and expresses a lasting bond
- infants express these feelings not only to
caregivers but also to favorite toys and to
siblings - Infants of this age smile more in the presence of
people, especially familiar people, and their
favorite objects - Smiling is related partly to emotion and partly
to communication with others - smiling is a reflection of a socially shared
emotion -
52Emotional Development
The Development of Enjoyment Affection
- Differential responsiveness to people on reunion
is probably one of the earliest and most reliable
ways of telling who the most important people in
a babys life are - In a stressful situation, if both the mother and
another caregiver are present, infants will
approach the mother - Even when infants spend more hours of the day in
the company of caregivers, as on an Israeli
kibbutz, they show more positive responses when
reunited with their mothers after a brief
separation than when reunited with their
metapelets, or caregivers - These findings suggest that babies have a growing
awareness of the specialness of certain people
and respond to them in ways that communicate the
depth of their feelings.
53Emotional Development
The Development of Mixed Emotions
- In one study, a stranger presented dolls and
teddy bears to infants of this age - the babies alternatively reached out toward the
objects and pulled their arms back - Another study found evidence for mixed emotions
by naturalistic observation in the infants homes
- this study found that pure expressions of joy,
anger, distress, excitement, or fear occur only
about half the time - the other half of the time the infants face
expresses more than one emotion
54Emotional Development
The Development of Mixed Emotions
- Enjoyment, for example, often is expressed with
elements of excitement or surprise, as when a
smile is combined with a jaw-drop, producing a
wide-open, smiling mouth - This is called a play smile
- play smiles in 12-month-olds occur during games
involving some physical activity or touch
combined with an element of excitement - These are games like tickle, chase, and tossing
the baby in the air - play smiles are more likely to occur in physical
games and thus appear more during father-infant
play than in mother-infant play - Smiles have also been observed to occur with nose
wrinkles, blinks, blows, and waves each
combination reflects a slight variation in the
meaning of the enjoyment
55Emotional Development
The Development of Mixed Emotions
- In an effort to regulate emotion, distress will
sometimes be combined with biting or stiffening
the lip, showing that the baby is trying to
control the crying - one-year-old babies often alternate between
distress and enjoyment, showing that they are
able to maintain interest in a toy or in social
interaction even though they may be tired or
frustrated - One-year-olds will sometimes look directly at the
caregiver when crying, or they may pull at the
caregiver, gesture to be picked up, or point to
the offending object or event - relaxed touching and holding by the caregiver can
provide encouragement for the infants efforts to
gain control over frustration, teaching the baby
the capacity to persist - the baby can learn that feeling distressed and
frustrated does not necessarily mean the end of
the gamethat these negative affects are both
tolerable and manageable
56Emotional Development
The Development of Mixed Emotions
- Another example of a mixed emotion appearing at
this age is jealousy - Year-old infants were observed while their
mothers and a strange adult female played with a
picture book or a child-sized doll - infants showed more protest and negative
vocalization and their play was more inhibited
primarily in the mother-with-doll condition - these responses were interpreted as jealousy
because the infant was not upset about the mother
with a book, nor about the strangers behavior - jealousy is an example of wanting to approach the
mother but avoiding her at the same time
57Emotional Development
The Development of Mixed Emotions
- Because of the emergence of mixed emotions, new
forms of parent-child play develop as a way to
help infants understand and cope with emotional
changes - Teasing games are especially important in this
regard - a tease takes something that has a serious
emotional tone and alters it into a more positive
tone - between 9 and 12 months, infants learn the art of
teasing and also become able to appreciate the
teases of others - the contradictory messages in these games provide
important opportunities for infants to learn how
to regulate their emotions, how to affect other
people, and also how to have - teasing is relatively more frequent in
father-infant than in mother-infant play
58Emotional Development
The Development of Mixed Emotions
- These findings show that infants are becoming
emotionally complex - Not only are they aware of the relationships
between objects and between events, they are also
aware of the relationships between their
different feelings
59Emotional Development
Individual Differences in Emotion Regulation
- Infants of this age are learning emotion
regulation to maintain some self-control in the
face of highly arousing (either exciting or
distressing) situations - At this age, infants show differences in how they
use people or objects to regulate emotions
60Emotional Development
Individual Differences in Emotion Regulation
- Heart-rate measures have shown that even when
infants do not cry during maternal separation,
they do get aroused - we may observe an extended glance at the door,
perhaps a sad expression, and then a concerted
effort to become involved with the toys - it is almost as if the babies were using the toys
to prevent feeling sad and lonely - other infants who may ignore their mother when
she returns from a separation also show elevated
heart rates - these babies are coping with their ambivalent
feelings about their mother in relation to the
feeling of loss they must have experienced when
she was out of the room - the manner in which the infants cope with these
feelings is not necessarily adaptive, since it
effectively removes them from the only source of
comfort they might receive their mothers
61Emotional Development
Individual Differences in Emotion Regulation
- Some infants are temperamentally more fearful and
withdrawn, and they are more likely to become
fearful or sad in stressful situations, such as
during separations and in the presence of
strangers or unusual situations - These differences in emotionality are associated
with individual differences in brain asymmetry - infants who become easily distressed to the point
of crying by a maternal separation have more
marked differences between left- and right-brain
activity in the frontal area - these findings show that the infants ability to
remain attentive to the situation, rather than to
withdraw, may be in part responsible for higher
levels of regulation of fear
62Emotional Development
Individual Differences in Emotion Regulation
- This suggests that there is a relationship
between cognition (attention) and emotion
regulation - some infants are more focused, attentive, and not
easily upset - others seem more emotional and require more
support from adults in challenging situations
63Emotional Development
Developments in the Infants Ability to Perceive
Emotion Intention Expressed by Other People
- By 6 or 7 months, infants are capable of
perceiving a few simple facial expressions - By 9 or 10 months, infants can distinguish more
expressions, and they are beginning to use the
emotional information displayed by others in a
meaningful manner - These developments in the perception of emotions
in other people are one of the ways in which the
infant is becoming increasingly aware of
relationships
64Emotional Development
Developments in the Infants Ability to Perceive
Emotion Intention Expressed by Other People
- Affective sharing occurs when infants wish to
communicate their feelings to another person or
to confirm their feelings with another person - between 9 and 12 months of age, affective sharing
is the most common response in social situations,
and it tends almost always to involve positive
emotions
65Emotional Development
Developments in the Infants Ability to Perceive
Emotion Intention Expressed by Other People
- Social referencing occurs when infants face an
uncertain situation. In this case, they look to
another persons emotional expressions to help
them decide what to do in this situation - between 10 and 12 months, infants begin to use
social referencing systematically, although
social referencing does not occur as frequently
as affective sharing in naturalistic situations - by 10 months, if mothers display a negative
expression, some infants will avoid crossing a
visual cliff, avoid playing with toys or avoid
approaching a rabbit in a cage, and they will
show more aversive responses to strangers to whom
the mothers show negative expressions - negative expressions seem to have a more powerful
effect on regulating infant behavior at this age - opposite patterns of behavior can be seen if the
mothers display positive expressions in these
situations
66Emotional Development
Developments in the Infants Ability to Perceive
Emotion Intention Expressed by Other People
- Underlying both affective sharing and social
referencing is the development of the ability to
delay an immediate emotional reaction to a
situation and to evaluate how one is going to
feel about the situation - Appraisal is the ability to use cognitive
comparisons of alternate interpretations to
regulate ones emotions - the appraisal at this age differs from the
appraisal seen during the period between 6 and 9
months - in that earlier period, infants are often
observed to show a slight delay in their
emotional reactions, as if deciding what to feel
in a particular situation - after 10 months the infant looks to another
person to decide what to feel
67Emotional Development
Developments in the Infants Ability to Perceive
Emotion Intention Expressed by Other People