Title: Short-term working memory
1Short-term working memory
- Students of memory (e.g., James, Galton) have
long considered that there is a memory system
that keeps in consciousness a small number of
ideas - William James referred to this system as primary
memory - the primary memory is probably more closely
related to working memory than to STM this model
will be discussed later on today
2Short-term working memory
- The capacity of short-term memory is
traditionally measured using a memory-span
procedure - in this procedure a participant is presented a
sequence of items, and is required to repeat them
back start with one item, increasing the number
of items by 1 until the participant begins to
make mistakes
3Short-term working memory
- the point at which the participant is able to
recall all items correctly 50 of the time is
designated as her/his memory span - factors affecting memory span
- auditory presentation leads to larger memory span
estimates than visual presentation - rhythmic presentation is better than non-rhythmic
presentation
4Short-term memory
- The next slide contains a series of digits. The
digits are presented in pairs. Read the pairs of
digits rhythmically aloud. Pause between each
pair. For example, suppose the digits were - 24 89 17 14 29 12 3
- After you have read the pairs aloud, I want you
to write down as many digits as you can remember.
Any questions?
5Read aloud these digits
6Short-term memory
- The next slide contains a series of digits. The
digits are presented in groups. Read groups of
digits aloud. Pause between each group. For
example, suppose the digits were - 248 917 142 9123
- After you have read the list aloud, I want you to
write down as many digits as you can remember.
Any questions?
7Read aloud these digits
8Short-term working memory
- factors affecting memory span (contd)
- recoding or chunking information George Miller
showed in his classic paper (1956) that memory
span is determined by the number of chunks or
integrated items you need to recall, not the
number of items presented -
9Inducing rapid forgetting
- Brown-Peterson paradigm
- Brown (1958) and Peterson Peterson (1959)
showed that it is possible to induce very rapid
forgetting if you distract person - paradigm
- study present a small number of items followed
by a number such as 632. Participant is required
to count backward by threes until given a recall
signal. Then he/she attempts to recall studied
items
10Inducing rapid forgetting
11Inducing rapid forgetting
- Note Murdock (1961) showed that performance is
about the same for 3 consonants as it is for 3
words, illustrating the importance of chunking - why is information forgotten in the
Brown-Peterson paradigm?
12Inducing rapid forgetting
- why is information forgotten in the
Brown-Peterson paradigm? - trace decay automatic fading of memory
- interference memory is disrupted by other memory
traces - proactive interference effects of prior items on
recall of subsequent items - retroactive interference effects of subsequent
items on recall of previous items
13Inducing rapid forgetting
- why is information forgotten in the
Brown-Peterson paradigm? - Petersons argued that it must be trace decay it
couldnt be retroactive interference because
numbers are very different from consonants - Keppel Underwood (1962) showed that proactive
interference seemed to be responsible because if
performance on the first trial only is examined
there is little decline in performance over the
retention interval
14Inducing rapid forgetting
- Further evidence for the importance of proactive
interference (PI) - release from PI
- numerous studies have established that if you
present several lists of items using a
Brown-Peterson procedure (Study present list of
3 items count backwards by 3s for 15 sec, then
attempt recall of the studied items. Results show
that performance declines across lists
15Inducing rapid forgetting
- Results show that performance declines across
lists (build up of PI) - If you change categories, then performance
increases (release from PI)
16One or two memory systems
- The theoretical question underlying much of this
research had to do with whether there was
evidence for the STM/LTM distinction - One approach to investigating this question
involves determining whether certain tasks have
separable components - One task is free recall
17Free Recall performance (Craik, 1970)
18Interpretation of free recall study
- Primacy and intermediate components of the serial
position curve are lower in the delayed compared
to immediate condition recency portion of the
curve is differentially lower in the delayed
condition - interpretation delayed condition has a stronger
influence on recency portion of curve because
recency reflects STM performance
19Neuropsychological Evidence for separation of STM
and LTM
- Data from amnesics support the viability of the
distinction between STM and LTM because amnesics
have normal digit span, which is mediated by STM,
but are impaired in their ability to acquire and
retain LTM memories -
20Neuropsychological Evidence for separation of STM
and LTM
- Free recall data in amnesics also supports this
distinction. Given your understanding of free
recall I want you to predict performance of
amnesics (Baddeley Warrington, 1970) - In immediate free recall, how should amnesics
perform on the recency portion of the curve? - What about the primacy portion of the curve?
21Short-term working memory
- Atkinson-Shiffrin model of memory (1968)
- distinguishes between two types of memory
short-term and long-term memory - short-term memory (STM) a temporary storage
system capable of holding a small amount of
information (e.g., telephone number) - information in STM is forgotten quickly unless it
is rehearsed or transferred into LTM - Long-term memory (LTM) a permanent memory store
with no capacity limitations
22Atkinson-Shiffrin model of memory
Rehearsal
Incoming information
Long-term memory
Short-term memory
Transfer
Information displaced
23Problems with modal model
- Modal model assumes that STS plays a critical
role in the transfer of information into LTS - Specifically, this model suggests that the
capacity of the STS should determine the
probability that an item enters LTS and - The amount of exposure in STS should affect the
likelihood that an item enters into LTS
24Problems with modal model
- Both these implications are incorrect
- several studies have shown that under some
conditions the number of times material is
rehearsed is a poor predictor that it will be
recalled subsequently (shallow rehearsal)
25Problems with modal model
- Shallice and Warrington (1970) and others have
established that at least some people with poor
memory span (this suggests that STS is damaged)
have normal long-term memory - KF memory span WAIS score 2, Mean 10,
Standard deviation 3 - established that KF understood spoken words by
presenting a list of spoken words task was to
tap table when words were from a given category - KF also was impaired when RN STM test administered
26Summary
- Evidence supporting STM vs LTM distinction
- tasks such as free recall seem to have both STM
and LTM components - Neuropsychological evidence suggests that both
components can be damaged - amnesics have damaged LTM component, but intact
STM component - KF (and others) have damaged STM but intact LTM
27Summary
- However, the modal model (Atkinson-Shiffrin) does
have problems accounting for - the finding that patients with STM deficits
appear to have intact LTM - maintaining an item in STM does not ensure its
transfer to LTM
28Working memory model of Baddeley
- Baddeleys early work focused on testing the
hypothesis that STS is important because it acts
as a working memory, a system that is important
for holding and manipulating information, and it
is needed for a broad range of cognitive tasks
29Working memory model of Baddeley
- Experimental paradigm (dual task paradigm)
- primary task grammatical reasoning
- Determine whether sentences are true/false
- e.g., A follows B -- BA (true)
- e.g., B is not preceded by A - AB (false)
- secondary task concurrent digit task remember
number sequences ranging in length from 0 to 8
30Baddeley (1986) contd
- Results
- as shown in the accompanying figure, reasoning
time increased with concurrent digit load.
However, performance remained high, and errors
remained low (about 4 and did not vary with
digit load) - thus, overall performance remains quite good,
even when the overall digit load is 8 (memory
span capacity)
31Baddeley (1986)
32Other important results
- Baddeley, Lewis, Eldridge, Thomson (1984)
showed that - a concurrent digit span task had a strong effect
on encoding and remembering new material - however, it had no effect on accuracy of
performance when the concurrent digit span task
was performed during retrieval (although
retrieval latency was slowed) - this suggests that the system responsible for
holding digits does not play a critical role in
retrieval as suggested by previous models of
memory
33Conclusions
- These findings and others are difficult to
reconcile with a model in which overloading the
short-term store leads to a complete breakdown of
performance on the primary task
34Working memory model of Baddeley
- Baddeley proposed to account for these results by
postulating that the digit span limitations are
set by one system, leaving other components of
working memory relatively unimpaired - Basic model of working memory consists of a
controlling attentional system (called the
central executive) and two slave systems, an
articulatory or phonological loop system and a
visuo-spatial sketch pad
35Baddeleys working memory model
Visuo-spatial sketchpad
Phonological loop
Central Executive
36Working memory
- Phonological loop characteristics
- consists of a phonological store (codes
speech-based information), and maintains
information for about 2 seconds - articulatory control process that refreshes items
in store by means of subvocal rehearsal
37Working memory
- Phonological loop
- appears to play an important role in reading
- poor readers tend to have poor short-term memory
span - also appears to play a role in the comprehension
of language and in the acquisition of vocabulary
38Visuo-spatial sketchpad
- Information can enter the sketchpad visually or
through the generation of a visual image - access to this store by visual information is
obligatory - the information in this store may be visual or
spatial or both
39Central Executive
- The central executive plays an important role in
controlling attention. Our discussion of the
central executive will begin with a discussion of
the interplay of attention and memory
40Central Executive
- Vigilance
- recall vigilance refers to sustained attention
- Parasuraman (1979) showed that vigilance
performance decreases if the vigilance task has a
short-term memory component involving storage and
manipulation of information. For example, if the
participant has to detect three consecutive odd
numbers from a stream of digits or must judge
whether adjacent items are of the same hue,
performance declines
41Central Executive
- Vigilance
- however, if the participant must evaluate each
item on its own (e.g., detect whether a product
such as a frying pan) has flaws, then performance
tends to remain stable - Dual task performance
- as discussed in a prior lecture, it is difficult
to perform two tasks at the same time. However,
the degree of difficulty depends upon the tasks
being performed and the expertise of the person
42Dual task performance
- Background
- its well established that Fitts Law, Fitts
Peterson (1964) that the time to strike an object
increases when the object is smaller or further
away - what happens when you have to strike an object
that is large and close with your right hand and
small and further away with your left hand?
43Dual task performance
- Result, Kelso, Southard, Goodman (1979)
- right and left hand hit the targets at the same
time - this suggests that hands do not function as
separate systems, but that their actions are
coordinated - in general people have difficulty emitting two
responses at the same time. This phenomenon is
known as refractoriness
44Dual task performance
- This phenomenon was used by Posner Boies (1971)
to investigate the attentional demands associated
with a letter matching task - Task 1
- Participant shown a warning signal, the the first
letter (A), and 1 sec later a second letter
(e.g., b). Participant must press one button if
the letters have the same name, and another
button (with their right hand) if the letters are
different
45Dual task performance
- Task 2
- Participant must listen for a clearly audible
tone, and must respond with left hand as quickly
as possible - primary dependent variable
- time it takes to respond to the auditory tone.
Using this procedure Posner Boies hoped to
investigate whether different aspects of the
primary task were more attentionally demanding
46Posner Boies (1971)
47Dual task performance
- Results
- as the Figure shows the time to respond to the
auditory signal was greatest at about the time
that the second letter was presented - Posner Boies interpreted this to mean that the
attentional demands on processing are greatest
when the second letter arrives and a decision
must be made - it is possible though that the result is
attributable to response interference
48Dual task performance
- Alternative interpretation
- it is possible though that the result is
attributable to response interference - McLeod (1978) replicated the Posner Boies study
only in the auditory condition participants
responded verbally by saying bip. Under these
conditions there was little slowing of the
response. This raises the possibility that the
Posner and Boies results are a consequence of the
type of response required
49Dual task performance
- Alternative interpretation of McLeods findings
- maybe making a bip response is automatic and
hence there is no cost associated with performing
it - McLeod and Posner (1984) performed an experiment
in which they contrasted a condition in which
responses could be made automatically using the
verbal articulatory loop and a second condition
in which verbal responses required controlled
processing
50Dual task performance
- Method
- Condition (automatic) probe stimuli up or
down. Say up or down - Condition (controlled) probe stimuli up or
down. Say high or low - Results
- in the automatic condition, response time to the
probe stimulus did not vary nearly as much as in
the controlled condition
51Dual task performance
- Conclusions
- it appears that two tasks can be performed well
at the same time provided that one task can be
performed relatively automatically - other examples Allport and Shaeffer (1972)
showed that skilled pianists can sight read and
play music and shadow a stream of prose (hear and
repeat back prose)
52Episodic buffer of working memory (Baddeleys new
model)
- Overview
- recently Baddeley updated the 3-component model
of working memory - It proposes a 4th component, an episodic buffer
- It has limited capacity
- Stores information in a multimodal code
- Binds information from subsidiary perceptual
systems and LTM into episodic memory - Information is consciously retrieved
53Episodic buffer of working memory (Baddeleys new
model)
- Background
- 3 component model of working memory consists of
central executive and two slave systems, the
phonological loop and the visuo-spatial sketchpad - Central executive is an attention controller
- Phonological loop stores speech-based info
- Visuospatial sketchpad stores visual info
54Episodic buffer of working memory (Baddeleys new
model)
- Problems with 3-component model of WM
- Articulatory suppression
- Saying the repetitively (occupying the
phonological loop) does not have a devastating
effect on recall of visually presented numbers - Recall drops from 7 to 5 digits
- One might expect recall to drop dramatically
because Phonological loop is occupied and VSS is
not very good at storing this type of information
55Episodic buffer of working memory (Baddeleys new
model)
- Problems with 3-component model of WM
- Prose recall of a patient (PV) with word-span of
1 word is 5 words. This is less than the span of
15 words, but much more than 1 words - Possible accounts
- 1. Sentences are stored in PVs LTM. Implausible
because PV has normal LTM. Also amnesic px have
normal memory span
56Episodic buffer of working memory
- Possible accounts
- 4. information is stored in an episodic memory
buffer separate from LTM - Accounts for this result
- Also accounts for finding that amnesics can
retain relatively large amounts of complex
information briefly (e.g., sentence span, info
about a bridge game) - People integrate information across modalities
(note may be two types of integration automatic
and controlled episodic integration is
controlled integration) see binding problem
discussion -
57Episodic buffer of working memory
- Binding problem
- Information that is processed independently by
separate cognitive processes must be bound
together because our experience of the world (and
our memory of it as well) is coherent - People can also retrieve information about an
episode when give part of an episode (e.g., given
a spatial cue, state what object was stored
there) - Episodic buffer is one way in which the binding
problem can be solved
584-component model of WM (see Fig.1)
Central Exec
visspat
Episodic Buff
Phon.
Episodic LTM
59Properties of Model
- See previous notes for description of
- Central Executive Function
- Phonological Loop
- Visual spatial sketchpad
60Properties of Model
- Episodic buffer
- Integrates information across modalities and from
different sources - Integrates information across time
- Has limited capacity
- Is capable of manipulating information
- Is consciously accessible from Central Executive
61A model of the Central ExecutiveSupervisory
Attentional System SAS
- Norman and Shallice developed a model of the
control of action called the Supervisory
Attentional System - this model was developed by considering our
knowledge of action slips and frontal lobe
function
62A model of the Central ExecutiveSupervisory
Attentional System SAS
- Action slips
- probably all of us have had the experience of
performing some unintended action - e.g., driving home from York in your car and
forgetting to make a detour to pick up your
clothes from the dry cleaners - e.g., William James going upstairs and ending up
in bed - Reason (1979) has studied action slips and showed
that these errors tend to occur when you are
pre-occupied with some other thought
63A model of the Central ExecutiveSupervisory
Attentional System SAS
- Action slips are actions that are inappropriate
for the goals of the participant. However, the
actions themselves are meaningful, and reasonably
well performed - my driving is safe, I obey traffic rules etc.
- This suggests that some actions, once they are
initiated, can be accurately performed with
little conscious attention being paid to them
64A model of the Central ExecutiveSupervisory
Attentional System SAS
- Other actions and other types of behaviour seem
to require a central system and performance
declines if such a system is not in place - research with damaged frontal lobe patients and
monkeys suggests that performance is impaired if
it requires - coordination of different elements of a complex
activity - focused attention
- focusing on the whole of a task
- working on new situations
65A model of the Central ExecutiveSupervisory
Attentional System SAS
- It is well established that patients with frontal
lobe damage may have relatively intact
performance on IQ tests - Luria (1966) proposed that the frontal lobes are
involved in programming, regulation, and
verification of activity
66A model of the Central ExecutiveSupervisory
Attentional System SAS
- Sample problem given to pt with frontal damage
- There were 18 books on two shelves, and there
were twice as many books on one shelf than on the
other. How many books were on each shelf? - Pt. Response
- Step 1. 18/2 9 (Clause 1)
- Step 2. 18 x 2 36 (Clause 2)
67A model of the Central ExecutiveSupervisory
Attentional System SAS
- For problems such as these Shallice, Norman, and
others have proposed that a central executive is
needed - their model is presented in the next slide
68Supervisory Attentional System
Trigger Data Base
Perceptual Structures
Effector System
Contention Scheduling
69SAS system
- According to this system routine actions run off
relatively automatically - perceptual information comes into the system and
it makes contact with stored information and that
information triggers certain responses. These
responses eventually result in actions that are
produced by the effector system - e.g., walking on a country road
70SAS system
- At any given moment this model postulates that
our behaviour is controlled by schemata, that
control lower-level programs - for example the schema that controls our driving
requires visual spatial and motor control
systems, and may call particular component schema
in well-defined circumstances (e.g., if light
turns orange, and you are well away from the
intersection, start braking)
71SAS system
- schemata are assumed to be activated by
triggering inputs, and to be selected if the
level of activation exceeds a threshold - they also tend to be mutually inhibitory
- once a schema is selected, the component schema
associated with a given schema become activated
(e.g., component schema for braking, turning on
lights, windshields etc.) - the process of routine selection between
alternative actions is called contention
scheduling
72SAS system
- the process of routine selection between
alternative actions is called contention
scheduling see Figure - e.g., light is orange and you are close to
intersection, do you brake, accelerate, or
maintain speed and continue through intersection
73SAS system
- in addition, this model assumes that there is an
additional system, the supervisory attentional
system - this system has access to the environment and to
the organisms intentions - it does not directly control behavior, but
instead modulates the lower level
contention-scheduling system by activating or
inhibiting particular schemata
74SAS system
- the supervisory attentional system is involved in
initiating willed actions, and in working in
situations in which routine actions are not
satisfactory--e.g., dealing with novelty,
overcoming temptation, etc.