Title: Kin Selection and Social Behavior
1Kin Selection and Social Behavior
2Types of social interactions among members of the
same species (Table 11.1)
- The actor in any social interaction affects the
recipient of the action as well as himself. The
costs and benefits of interactions are measured
in units of surviving offspring (fitness).
Actor benefits Actor is harmed
Recipient benefits Cooperative Altruistic
Recipient is harmed Selfish Spiteful
3Kin selection and altruistic behavior
- For Darwin, the apparent existence of altruism
presented a special difficulty, which at first
appeared to me insuperable, and actually fatal to
my whole theory. - However, he also suggested a solution selection
might favor traits that decreased the fitness of
the actor if they increased the reproductive
success of close relatives - This form of selection, which takes into account
the fitness benefits to relatives is kin selection
4Hamiltons Rule
- William Hamilton (1964) developed a genetic model
showing that an altruistic allele could increase
in frequency if the following condition is
satisfied - Br - C gt 0
- Where B is the benefit to the recipient and C is
the cost to the actor, both being measured in
units of surviving offspring, and r is the
coefficient of relatedness (or relationship)
between actor and recipient
5Inclusive fitness
- Hamilton introduced the concept of inclusive
fitness, which includes direct fitness indirect
fitness - Direct fitness is personal reproduction
- Indirect fitness is the additional reproduction
of relatives that is made possible by an
individuals actions
6The coefficient of relatedness, r
- The proportion of alleles in two individuals that
are identical by descent (ibd) - The coefficient of relatedness of full sibs is r
0.5 - To see why this is so, we can use the following
example, in which we give each of the four
alleles at a locus in the two parents a unique
label
7Proportion of alleles shared ibd by full sibs
- P A1A2 x A3A4
- O
- The average proportion of alleles shared ibd by
pairs of full sibs is 0.5
Sib 1 Sib 1 Sib 1 Sib 1
A1A3 A2A3 A1A4 A2A4
Sib 2 A1A3 1.0 0.5 0.5 0
Sib 2 A2A3 0.5 1.0 0 0.5
Sib 2 A1A4 0.5 0 1.0 0.5
Sib 2 A2A4 0 0.5 0.5 1.0
8Some coefficients of relatedness
- Parent to offspring, r 1/2
- Full sibs, r 1/2
- Half sibs, r 1/4
- First cousins, r 1/8
- Grandparent to grandchild, r 1/4
- Aunt or uncle to niece or nephew, r 1/4
9Altruistic behavior in Beldings ground squirrels
- Breed in colonies
- Male offspring disperse far from native burrow
- Female offspring tend to remain and breed close
by. Therefore, females in proximity tend to be
closely related - Squirrels give alarm calls when predators are
spotted (different calls for mammalian predators
vs. birds of prey) - Is alarm calling altruistic and can it be
understood as a result of kin selection?
10In ground squirrels most alarm calling is done by
females (Sherman 1977) (Fig. 11.2 b)
- Based on 102 encounters with predatory mammals
- Blue line is expected frequency if each type of
individual called in proportion to the number of
times it was present when a predator appeared - Mortality is 8 for calling individual vs. 4 for
non-callers when predator is a mammal
11Female ground squirrels are more likely to give
alarm calls when close kin are nearby (Sherman
1977) (Fig. 11.3)
- Based on 119 encounters with predatory mammals
- Blue line is expected frequency if each type of
pairing produced calls in proportion to the
number of times it was present when a predator
appeared
12Nest helping in white-fronted bee-eaters
- In white-fronted bee-eaters (and some other birds
where breeding opportunities are extremely
restricted), young adults often forego their own
reproduction to help at the nests of other
individuals. - This is clearly altruistic. Evidence suggests
that nest helping can be explained by kin
selection
13White-fronted bee-eater (Merops bullockoides)
14In bee-eaters, helpers assist close relatives
(Emlen and Wrege 1988) (Fig.
- Among non-breeders, those born in a clan are much
more likely to be nest helpers than those who
enter a clan from outside and are unrelated to
offspring being raised in that season - Nest helping is disproportionately directed
toward close relatives
15Nest helpers increase the number of young birds
fledged (Emlen and Wrege 1991) (Fig. 11.7)
- A group size of 2 represents a pair without
helpers. The average number of young fledged
0.51 for pairs - Each additional helper at the nest increases the
number of young fledged by 0.47 on average
16Kin-selected discrimination in cannibalistic
spadefoot toad tadpoles (Pfennig 1999) (Fig. 11.8
a, b)
- Tadpoles develop into typical morphs that eat
mostly decaying plant matter or into carnivores
that eat other tadpoles. - Carnivores are more likely to eat non-sibs than
sibs when given a choice between one of each kind
17Kin selection can explain the presence of
discrimination between sibs and non-sibs in
cannibalistic tiger salamander larvae (Pfennig et
al. 1999) (Fig. 11.8c)
Benefit B 2
Cost C 0
Experiment consisted of placing 1 predatory morph
6 sibling typical morphs 18 non-sibling
typical morphs in each of 18 enclosures in
natural pond.
18Coots can avoid parasitic altruism (helping
non-kin) (Lyon 2003) (Fig. 11.10 c, d)
- If selection can favor helping kin, it should
also favor avoiding sacrifices for non-kin - Coots that accept eggs from other birds lose 1
offspring of their own for each parasitic
offspring - Coots that reject parasitic eggs have the same
number of offspring as unparasitized birds
(dashed line)
19Eusociality the ultimate in reproductive altruism
- Characteristics of eusociality
- Overlap in generations between parents and
offspring - Cooperative brood care
- Specialized castes of non-reproductive
individuals - Insects (termites, hymenoptera), snapping shrimp,
naked mole rats
20Haplodiploidy and eusociality in hymenoptera
(bees, wasps, ants)
- Males are haploid (develop from unfertilized
eggs) and females are diploid - Hamilton (1972) proposed that haplodiploidy
predisposes hymenoptera to eusociality because
females are more closely related to one another
(r 3/4) than they are to their own offspring (r
1/2) - Females may maximize inclusive fitness by being
sterile workers and helping to produce
reproductive sisters (rather than by being
reproductives themselves)
21Proportion of alleles shared ibd by sisters in a
haplodiploid species
- P A1A2 x A3
- O
- The average proportion of alleles shared ibd by
pairs of full sibs is 0.75
Sister 1 Sister 1
A1A3 A2A3
Sister 2 A1A3 1.0 0.5
Sister 2 A2A3 0.5 1.0
22Is haplodiploidy the explanation of eusociality
in hymenoptera?
- Probably not
- The preceding analysis assumed only 1 male
fertilizes a queen this is not true in
honeybees, for example - In some species, colonies may be founded by more
than 1 queen - Many eusocial non-hymenoptera are diploid (e.g.,
termites) - Many hymenoptera are not eusocial (eusociality
may have three independent origins associated
with nest-building and the need to supply larvae
with food) - Haplodiploidy may facilitate the evolution of
eusociality but a more important factor may be
the need for help in rearing young
23Sociality and nesting behavior in hymenoptera
(Hunt 1999) (Fig. 11.13
- Families that include eusocial species are
indicated in boldface type
24Naked mole rats
- All young in a colony produced by a single queen
and 2 3 reproductive males - Not haplodiploid, but colony members are highly
inbred (average r 0.81) - 85 of matings are between full-sibs or parents
and offspring - Queens use physical dominance to coerce help from
less closely related individuals
25Naked mole rat queens preferentially shove
nonrelatives (Reeve and Sherman 1991) (Fig. 11.16)
26Parent offspring conflict
- Parental care is a special case of providing
fitness benefits for close relatives - The offspring is the fitness of the parent (this
means that benefits and costs of parental care
both accrue to the parent) - In species that provide extensive parental care,
the fitness benefit to the parent of providing
additional care to current offspring needs to be
weighed against the fitness cost of that
additional care in terms of lost future offspring
27Parent offspring conflict occurs because
parents and offspring value the costs of parental
care differently (Trivers 1985) (Fig 11.18)
- As offspring grow, the benefit/cost ratio for the
parent declines. Benefit (B) is measured in
terms of increased survival of offspring
receiving parental care cost (C) is measured in
terms of lost future offspring due to continued
parental care. From parents point of view, it
should stop giving parental care when B/C
declines to 1. - But, the offspring devalues the cost to the
parent by 1/2 because lost future full-sibs are
related to the offspring by r 1/2. Therefore,
the offspring wants parental care to continue
until the B/C ratio for the parent is 1/2 - fig.
(a) (or 1/4 if future offspring are half-sibs -
fig. (b)
28Harassment in white-fronted bee-eaters can also
be analyzed in the context of parent-offspring
conflict and kin selection
- Fathers occasionally coerce sons into helping to
raise their siblings by harassing sons who are
trying to raise their own young - Sons are as closely related to their full sibs as
they are to their own offspring (r 1/2 in both
cases) - Furthermore the average number of offspring in
nests without helpers is 0.51, whereas every
helper increases the number of surviving
nestlings by 0.47 on average - Therefore, the direct fitness lost by a son who
is coerced into helping his parents is balanced
by the increase in indirect fitness that results
from helping his parents (provided that the son
would not have had helpers)
29Bee-eaters recruit helpers who are younger and
closely related (Emlen and Wrege 1992) (Fig.
11.19 b)
30Reciprocal altruism
- Reciprocation is offered to explain altruism
between unrelated individuals - The necessary conditions for reciprocal altruism
to evolve are - The fitness cost to the actor must be the
fitness benefit to the recipient - Non-reciprocators must be punished in some way
(otherwise alleles that caused cheating would
displace alleles for altruism) - Conditions that favor the evolution of reciprocal
altruism are - Stable social groups (so that individuals are
involved in repeated interactions with one
another) - Lots of opportunities for altruistic
interactions during an individuals lifetime - Good memory
- Symmetry of interactions between potential
altruists
31Blood-sharing in vampire bats an example of
reciprocal altruism? (Wilkinson 1984) 1
- Reciprocal altruism has been difficult to
document - In vampire bats (Desmodus rotundus) the basic
social unit is 8 12 females and their dependent
offspring that frequently roost together - Many individuals preferentially associate with
one another when roosting - The altruistic act is sharing blood meals by
regurgitation - 33 of young and 7 of adults fail to get a blood
meal on any given night - Bats are likely to starve to death if they go
three consecutive nights without a meal
32Blood-sharing in vampire bats an example of
reciprocal altruism? (Wilkinson 1984) 2
- Bats are more likely to share blood with
relatives and with associates. Both effects
(relationship and association) were statistically
significant. (These data do not include
regurgitation by mother to child because that is
parental care.) - Blood sharing was not random in a group of
captive individuals. Bats were more likely to
receive blood from and individual that they had
fed previously.
33Association, relatedness and altruism in vampire
bats (Wilkinson 1984)