Kin Selection and Social Behavior - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

About This Presentation
Title:

Kin Selection and Social Behavior

Description:

The actor in any social interaction affects the recipient of the action as well as himself. ... The fitness cost to the actor must be the fitness benefit to ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:127
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 34
Provided by: Bigp6
Learn more at: https://jan.ucc.nau.edu
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Kin Selection and Social Behavior


1
Kin Selection and Social Behavior
  • Chapter 11

2
Types of social interactions among members of the
same species (Table 11.1)
  • The actor in any social interaction affects the
    recipient of the action as well as himself. The
    costs and benefits of interactions are measured
    in units of surviving offspring (fitness).

Actor benefits Actor is harmed
Recipient benefits Cooperative Altruistic
Recipient is harmed Selfish Spiteful
3
Kin selection and altruistic behavior
  • For Darwin, the apparent existence of altruism
    presented a special difficulty, which at first
    appeared to me insuperable, and actually fatal to
    my whole theory.
  • However, he also suggested a solution selection
    might favor traits that decreased the fitness of
    the actor if they increased the reproductive
    success of close relatives
  • This form of selection, which takes into account
    the fitness benefits to relatives is kin selection

4
Hamiltons Rule
  • William Hamilton (1964) developed a genetic model
    showing that an altruistic allele could increase
    in frequency if the following condition is
    satisfied
  • Br - C gt 0
  • Where B is the benefit to the recipient and C is
    the cost to the actor, both being measured in
    units of surviving offspring, and r is the
    coefficient of relatedness (or relationship)
    between actor and recipient

5
Inclusive fitness
  • Hamilton introduced the concept of inclusive
    fitness, which includes direct fitness indirect
    fitness
  • Direct fitness is personal reproduction
  • Indirect fitness is the additional reproduction
    of relatives that is made possible by an
    individuals actions

6
The coefficient of relatedness, r
  • The proportion of alleles in two individuals that
    are identical by descent (ibd)
  • The coefficient of relatedness of full sibs is r
    0.5
  • To see why this is so, we can use the following
    example, in which we give each of the four
    alleles at a locus in the two parents a unique
    label

7
Proportion of alleles shared ibd by full sibs
  • P A1A2 x A3A4
  • O
  • The average proportion of alleles shared ibd by
    pairs of full sibs is 0.5

Sib 1 Sib 1 Sib 1 Sib 1
A1A3 A2A3 A1A4 A2A4
Sib 2 A1A3 1.0 0.5 0.5 0
Sib 2 A2A3 0.5 1.0 0 0.5
Sib 2 A1A4 0.5 0 1.0 0.5
Sib 2 A2A4 0 0.5 0.5 1.0
8
Some coefficients of relatedness
  • Parent to offspring, r 1/2
  • Full sibs, r 1/2
  • Half sibs, r 1/4
  • First cousins, r 1/8
  • Grandparent to grandchild, r 1/4
  • Aunt or uncle to niece or nephew, r 1/4

9
Altruistic behavior in Beldings ground squirrels
  • Breed in colonies
  • Male offspring disperse far from native burrow
  • Female offspring tend to remain and breed close
    by. Therefore, females in proximity tend to be
    closely related
  • Squirrels give alarm calls when predators are
    spotted (different calls for mammalian predators
    vs. birds of prey)
  • Is alarm calling altruistic and can it be
    understood as a result of kin selection?

10
In ground squirrels most alarm calling is done by
females (Sherman 1977) (Fig. 11.2 b)
  • Based on 102 encounters with predatory mammals
  • Blue line is expected frequency if each type of
    individual called in proportion to the number of
    times it was present when a predator appeared
  • Mortality is 8 for calling individual vs. 4 for
    non-callers when predator is a mammal

11
Female ground squirrels are more likely to give
alarm calls when close kin are nearby (Sherman
1977) (Fig. 11.3)
  • Based on 119 encounters with predatory mammals
  • Blue line is expected frequency if each type of
    pairing produced calls in proportion to the
    number of times it was present when a predator
    appeared

12
Nest helping in white-fronted bee-eaters
  • In white-fronted bee-eaters (and some other birds
    where breeding opportunities are extremely
    restricted), young adults often forego their own
    reproduction to help at the nests of other
    individuals.
  • This is clearly altruistic. Evidence suggests
    that nest helping can be explained by kin
    selection

13
White-fronted bee-eater (Merops bullockoides)
14
In bee-eaters, helpers assist close relatives
(Emlen and Wrege 1988) (Fig.
  1. Among non-breeders, those born in a clan are much
    more likely to be nest helpers than those who
    enter a clan from outside and are unrelated to
    offspring being raised in that season
  2. Nest helping is disproportionately directed
    toward close relatives

15
Nest helpers increase the number of young birds
fledged (Emlen and Wrege 1991) (Fig. 11.7)
  • A group size of 2 represents a pair without
    helpers. The average number of young fledged
    0.51 for pairs
  • Each additional helper at the nest increases the
    number of young fledged by 0.47 on average

16
Kin-selected discrimination in cannibalistic
spadefoot toad tadpoles (Pfennig 1999) (Fig. 11.8
a, b)
  • Tadpoles develop into typical morphs that eat
    mostly decaying plant matter or into carnivores
    that eat other tadpoles.
  • Carnivores are more likely to eat non-sibs than
    sibs when given a choice between one of each kind

17
Kin selection can explain the presence of
discrimination between sibs and non-sibs in
cannibalistic tiger salamander larvae (Pfennig et
al. 1999) (Fig. 11.8c)
Benefit B 2
Cost C 0
Experiment consisted of placing 1 predatory morph
6 sibling typical morphs 18 non-sibling
typical morphs in each of 18 enclosures in
natural pond.
18
Coots can avoid parasitic altruism (helping
non-kin) (Lyon 2003) (Fig. 11.10 c, d)
  • If selection can favor helping kin, it should
    also favor avoiding sacrifices for non-kin
  • Coots that accept eggs from other birds lose 1
    offspring of their own for each parasitic
    offspring
  • Coots that reject parasitic eggs have the same
    number of offspring as unparasitized birds
    (dashed line)

19
Eusociality the ultimate in reproductive altruism
  • Characteristics of eusociality
  • Overlap in generations between parents and
    offspring
  • Cooperative brood care
  • Specialized castes of non-reproductive
    individuals
  • Insects (termites, hymenoptera), snapping shrimp,
    naked mole rats

20
Haplodiploidy and eusociality in hymenoptera
(bees, wasps, ants)
  • Males are haploid (develop from unfertilized
    eggs) and females are diploid
  • Hamilton (1972) proposed that haplodiploidy
    predisposes hymenoptera to eusociality because
    females are more closely related to one another
    (r 3/4) than they are to their own offspring (r
    1/2)
  • Females may maximize inclusive fitness by being
    sterile workers and helping to produce
    reproductive sisters (rather than by being
    reproductives themselves)

21
Proportion of alleles shared ibd by sisters in a
haplodiploid species
  • P A1A2 x A3
  • O
  • The average proportion of alleles shared ibd by
    pairs of full sibs is 0.75

Sister 1 Sister 1
A1A3 A2A3
Sister 2 A1A3 1.0 0.5
Sister 2 A2A3 0.5 1.0
22
Is haplodiploidy the explanation of eusociality
in hymenoptera?
  • Probably not
  • The preceding analysis assumed only 1 male
    fertilizes a queen this is not true in
    honeybees, for example
  • In some species, colonies may be founded by more
    than 1 queen
  • Many eusocial non-hymenoptera are diploid (e.g.,
    termites)
  • Many hymenoptera are not eusocial (eusociality
    may have three independent origins associated
    with nest-building and the need to supply larvae
    with food)
  • Haplodiploidy may facilitate the evolution of
    eusociality but a more important factor may be
    the need for help in rearing young

23
Sociality and nesting behavior in hymenoptera
(Hunt 1999) (Fig. 11.13
  • Families that include eusocial species are
    indicated in boldface type

24
Naked mole rats
  • All young in a colony produced by a single queen
    and 2 3 reproductive males
  • Not haplodiploid, but colony members are highly
    inbred (average r 0.81)
  • 85 of matings are between full-sibs or parents
    and offspring
  • Queens use physical dominance to coerce help from
    less closely related individuals

25
Naked mole rat queens preferentially shove
nonrelatives (Reeve and Sherman 1991) (Fig. 11.16)
26
Parent offspring conflict
  • Parental care is a special case of providing
    fitness benefits for close relatives
  • The offspring is the fitness of the parent (this
    means that benefits and costs of parental care
    both accrue to the parent)
  • In species that provide extensive parental care,
    the fitness benefit to the parent of providing
    additional care to current offspring needs to be
    weighed against the fitness cost of that
    additional care in terms of lost future offspring

27
Parent offspring conflict occurs because
parents and offspring value the costs of parental
care differently (Trivers 1985) (Fig 11.18)
  • As offspring grow, the benefit/cost ratio for the
    parent declines. Benefit (B) is measured in
    terms of increased survival of offspring
    receiving parental care cost (C) is measured in
    terms of lost future offspring due to continued
    parental care. From parents point of view, it
    should stop giving parental care when B/C
    declines to 1.
  • But, the offspring devalues the cost to the
    parent by 1/2 because lost future full-sibs are
    related to the offspring by r 1/2. Therefore,
    the offspring wants parental care to continue
    until the B/C ratio for the parent is 1/2 - fig.
    (a) (or 1/4 if future offspring are half-sibs -
    fig. (b)

28
Harassment in white-fronted bee-eaters can also
be analyzed in the context of parent-offspring
conflict and kin selection
  • Fathers occasionally coerce sons into helping to
    raise their siblings by harassing sons who are
    trying to raise their own young
  • Sons are as closely related to their full sibs as
    they are to their own offspring (r 1/2 in both
    cases)
  • Furthermore the average number of offspring in
    nests without helpers is 0.51, whereas every
    helper increases the number of surviving
    nestlings by 0.47 on average
  • Therefore, the direct fitness lost by a son who
    is coerced into helping his parents is balanced
    by the increase in indirect fitness that results
    from helping his parents (provided that the son
    would not have had helpers)

29
Bee-eaters recruit helpers who are younger and
closely related (Emlen and Wrege 1992) (Fig.
11.19 b)
30
Reciprocal altruism
  • Reciprocation is offered to explain altruism
    between unrelated individuals
  • The necessary conditions for reciprocal altruism
    to evolve are
  • The fitness cost to the actor must be the
    fitness benefit to the recipient
  • Non-reciprocators must be punished in some way
    (otherwise alleles that caused cheating would
    displace alleles for altruism)
  • Conditions that favor the evolution of reciprocal
    altruism are
  • Stable social groups (so that individuals are
    involved in repeated interactions with one
    another)
  • Lots of opportunities for altruistic
    interactions during an individuals lifetime
  • Good memory
  • Symmetry of interactions between potential
    altruists

31
Blood-sharing in vampire bats an example of
reciprocal altruism? (Wilkinson 1984) 1
  • Reciprocal altruism has been difficult to
    document
  • In vampire bats (Desmodus rotundus) the basic
    social unit is 8 12 females and their dependent
    offspring that frequently roost together
  • Many individuals preferentially associate with
    one another when roosting
  • The altruistic act is sharing blood meals by
    regurgitation
  • 33 of young and 7 of adults fail to get a blood
    meal on any given night
  • Bats are likely to starve to death if they go
    three consecutive nights without a meal

32
Blood-sharing in vampire bats an example of
reciprocal altruism? (Wilkinson 1984) 2
  • Bats are more likely to share blood with
    relatives and with associates. Both effects
    (relationship and association) were statistically
    significant. (These data do not include
    regurgitation by mother to child because that is
    parental care.)
  • Blood sharing was not random in a group of
    captive individuals. Bats were more likely to
    receive blood from and individual that they had
    fed previously.

33
Association, relatedness and altruism in vampire
bats (Wilkinson 1984)
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com