Title: Kin Selection and Social Behavior
1Kin Selection and Social Behavior
- Interactions between individuals can have 4
possible outcomes in terms of fitness gains for
the participants.
2Kin Selection and Social Behavior
- Cooperation (mutualism) fitness gains for both
participants. - Altruism instigator pays fitness cost, recipient
benefits. - Selfishness instigator gains benefit, other
individual pays cost. - Spite both individuals suffer a fitness cost.
3Kin Selection and Social Behavior
- No clear cut cases of spite documented. The
behavior clearly harms the instigator for no
benefit so difficult to see how it could be
favored by selection. - Selfish and cooperative behaviors easily
explained by selection theory because they
benefit the instigator.
4The puzzle of altruism
- Altruism is hard to explain because the
instigator pays a cost and another individual
benefits. - How can selection favor the spread of an
altruistic allele that produces a behavior that
benefits other individuals at the expense of
individuals bearing the altruistic allele?
5- BTW when I say an allele that produces a
behavior I dont mean that the allele literally
directly codes for the behavior. - An allele directly code for the structure of
proteins. - However, proteins interact with other proteins
and the result of all these biochemical
interactions in building a brain can result in an
organism with one version of an allele behaving
somewhat differently from an individual with a
different version.
6The puzzle of altruism
- For Darwin altruism presented a special
difficulty, which at first appears to me
insuperable, and actually fatal to my whole
theory. - Darwin suggested however that if a behavior
benefited relatives, it might be favored by
selection.
7The puzzle of altruism
- W.D. Hamilton (1964) developed a model that
showed how an allele that favored altruistic
behavior could spread under certain conditions.
8Coefficient of relatedness
- Key parameter is the coefficient of relatedness
r. - r is the probability that the homologous alleles
in two individuals are identical by descent (see
earlier notes on inbreeding for the concept of
identical by descentbasically copies of an
allele being inherited from a particular
individual).
9Calculating r
- Use a pedigree to calculate r.
- Pedigree shows all possible direct routes of
hereditary connection between the two
individuals. - Because parents contribute half their genes to
each offspring, the probability that alleles are
identical by descent for each step is 50 or 0.5.
10Calculating r
- To calculate r
- (i) Trace each unique path between the two
individuals via common ancestors and count the
number of steps needed. - (ii) For this path r 0.5 (number of steps).
Thus, if two steps r for this path 0.5 (2)
0.25. - (iii) To calculate final value of r you add
together the r values calculated from each path.
11(No Transcript)
12(No Transcript)
13(No Transcript)
14Hamiltons rule
- Given r the coefficient of relatedness between
the actor and the recipient, Hamiltons rule
states that an allele for altruistic behavior
will spread if - Br - C gt0
- Where B is benefit to recipient and C is the cost
to the actor. Unit of measurement for B and C is
surviving offspring.
15Hamiltons rule
- Altruistic behaviors are most likely to spread
when costs (C) are low, benefits (B) to recipient
are high, and the participants are closely
related (r is large).
16Applying Hamiltons rule
- You have a food item that is worth 2 units of
benefit to you. - You have a tiny nephew for whom the food would be
worth 10 units of benefit. - Should you eat the food or give it to your nephew?
17Applying Hamiltons rule
- The value of r for a nephew is ¼.
- Cost to you would be 2 (as youre giving up 2
units of benefit). - Benefit to nephew is 10.
- Is Br - C gt0?
- 10(¼) 2 ?
- 2.5 - 2 0.5. This is gt 0 so you should give the
food to your nephew. - Should you share with a cousin? r 1/8 for a
cousin.
18- Share with a cousin?
- No because Br - C is not gt 0.
- 10 1/8 2 1.25 2 -0.75
19Inclusive fitness
- Hamilton invented the idea of inclusive fitness
which divides an individuals fitness into two
components - Direct fitness results from an individuals
personal reproduction (the babies it produces) - Indirect fitness results from additional
reproduction by relatives, that is made possible
by an individuals actions. For example an
individual might help feed its sisters offspring
or guard them from predators.
20Kin selection
- Natural selection favoring the spread of alleles
that increase the indirect component of fitness
is called kin selection.
21Alarm calling in Beldings Ground Squirrels
- Giving alarm calls alerts other individuals but
may attract a predators attention. - Beldings Ground Squirrels give two different
calls depending on whether predator is a
predatory mammal (trill) or a hawk (whistle
Sherman 1985).
22http//michaelfrye.com/yosemite-journal/?p375
23Is alarm calling altruistic?
- Sherman and colleagues observed 256 natural
predator attacks.
https//www.flickr.com/photos/charlespan/652746514
5/
24Beldings Ground Squirrels
- In hawk attacks whistling squirrel is killed 2
of the time whereas non-whistling squirrels are
killed 28 of the time. - Calling squirrel appears to reduce its chance of
being killed.
25Beldings Ground Squirrels
- In predatory mammal attacks trilling squirrel is
killed 8 of the time and a non-trilling squirrel
is killed 4 of the time.
http//captainkimo.com/coyote-on-the-hunt-pouncing
-for-prey-at-yellowstone-national-park/
26Beldings Ground Squirrels
- Calling squirrel thus appears to increase its
risk of predation. - Whistling appears to be selfish, but trilling
altruistic.
27Beldings Ground Squirrels
- Beldings Ground Squirrels breed in colonies in
Alpine meadows. - Males disperse, but female offspring tend to
remain and breed close by. Thus, females in
colony tend to be related.
28Beldings Ground Squirrels
- Sherman had pedigrees that showed relatedness
among his study animals. - Analysis of who called showed that females were
much more likely to call than males.
29(No Transcript)
30Beldings Ground Squirrels
- Females were also more likely than males to call
when they had relatives within earshot.
31(No Transcript)
32Beldings Ground Squirrels
- Relatives also cooperated in behaviors besides
alarm calling. - Females were much more likely to join close
relatives in chasing away trespassing ground
squirrels than less closely related kin and
non-kin.
33(No Transcript)
34Beldings Ground Squirrels
- Overall, data show that altruistic behavior is
not randomly directed. - It is focused on close relatives and should
result in indirect fitness gains by increasing
the survival prospects of these relatives and
hence their future reproduction.
35Kin selection and cannibalism in tadpoles
- Spadefoot toad tadpoles come in two morphs.
- Typical morph is omnivorous mainly eats decaying
plant material. - Cannibalistic morph has bigger jaws and catches
prey including other spadefoot tadpoles.
36Kin selection and cannibalism in tadpoles
- Pfennig (1999) tested whether cannibals
discriminate between kin and non-kin. - Placed 28 cannibalistic tadpoles in individual
containers. Added two omnivorous tadpoles
(tadpoles had never seen before) to each
container. One was a sibling, the other non-kin.
37Kin selection and cannibalism in tadpoles
- Pfenning waited until cannibal ate one tadpole,
then determined which had been eaten. - Found that kin were significantly less likely to
be eaten. Only 6 of 28 kin were eaten, but 22 of
28 non-kin.
38(No Transcript)
39Kin selection and cannibalism in tadpoles
- Pfennig also studied tiger salamanders whose
tadpoles also develop into cannibalistic morphs. - Kept 18 cannibals in separate enclosures in
natural pond. To each enclosure added 6 siblings
and 18 non-kin typical morph tadpoles.
40Kin selection and cannibalism in tadpoles
- Some cannibals discriminated between kin and
non-kin. Others did not. - Degree of relatedness to siblings 1/2
41Kin selection and cannibalism in tadpoles
- Thus, by Hamiltons rule discrimination in favor
of kin favored if B(r) - C gt 0 - Benefit estimated by counting number of siblings
that survived. Siblings of discriminating
cannibals twice as likely to survive as siblings
of non-discriminating cannibals.
42Kin selection and cannibalism in tadpoles
- Benefit thus approximately 2.
- Cost assessed by evaluating effect of not eating
siblings by comparing growth of discriminating
and non-discriminating cannibals. No difference
in growth rates. Cost then estimated as close to
0.
43(No Transcript)
44Kin selection and cannibalism in tadpoles
- By Hamiltons rule discrimination should be
favored because 2(1/2) - 0 1 which is gt0.
45Altruistic sperm in wood mice
- Moore et al. have demonstrated altruistic
behavior by sperm of European wood mice. - Females highly promiscuous. Males have large
testes and engage in intense sperm competition
with other males.
46Altruistic sperm in wood mice
- Wood mice sperm have hooks on their heads. And
connect together to form long trains of sperm
that can include thousands of sperm. - Swimming together sperm travel twice as fast as
if they swam separately.
47http//phenomena.nationalgeographic.com/2014/07/23
/these-mice-excel-at-assembling-the-ideal-sperm-sw
im-teams/
48(No Transcript)
49Altruistic sperm in wood mice
- To fertilize egg, train must break up.
- To break up train many sperm have to undergo
acrosome reaction releasing enzymes that usually
help fertilize an egg.
50Altruistic sperm in wood mice
- Releasing these enzymes before reaching an egg
means these sperm cannot fertilize the egg.
These sperm sacrifice themselves. - Because other sperm carry half of the same
alleles, sacrifice makes sense in terms of kin
selection.
51Discrimination against non-kin eggs by coots
- Important to avoid paying costs on behalf of
non-kin. - Lyon (2003) studied defense against nest
parasitism in American coots. - Coots often lay eggs in other coots nests in
hopes of having them reared.
52Discrimination against non-kin eggs by coots
- Accepting parasitic eggs is costly because half
of all chicks starve and same number reared in
parasitized and non-parasitized nests. - Thus, host parent loses one offspring for every
successful parasite.
53Discrimination against non-kin eggs by coots
- Because of high cost of being parasitized and
lack of benefit (assuming parasites are non-kin)
Hamiltons rule predicts coots should
discriminate against parasitic eggs. - Coot eggs very variable in appearance. If 2 eggs
laid within 24 hours Lyon knew one was a parasite.
54Discrimination against non-kin eggs by coots
- Among 133 hosts 43 rejected one or more
parasitic eggs. Rejected eggs differed from
hosts eggs significantly more than did accepted
eggs.
55(No Transcript)
56Discrimination against non-kin eggs by coots
- Females who accepted eggs laid one fewer egg of
their own for each parasitic egg they accepted.
Average total clutch (including parasites) 8
eggs,
57(No Transcript)
58Discrimination against non-kin eggs by coots
- Females who rejected eggs laid an average of 8 of
their own eggs even though they waited to finish
laying before disposing of eggs they were
rejecting. Coots can count! - By counting eggs and rejecting extras that do not
look right coots prevent themselves from being
parasitized.
59(No Transcript)
60Parent-offspring conflict.
- Parental care is an obvious form of altruism. In
many species parents invest huge quantities of
resources in their offspring. - Initially, parent and offspring agree that
investment in the offspring is worthwhile because
it enhances the offsprings prospects of survival
and reproduction.
61Parent-offspring conflict.
- However, a parent shares only 50 of its genes
with the offspring and is equally related to all
of its offspring, whereas offspring is 100
related to itself, but only shares 50 of genes
with its siblings. - As a result, at some point a parent will prefer
to reserve investment for future offspring rather
than investing in the current one, while the
current offspring will disagree. This leads to a
period of conflict called weaning.
62Parent-offspring conflict.
- The period of weaning conflict ends when both
offspring and parent agree that future investment
by the parent would be better directed at future
offspring. This is when the benefit to cost
ratio drops below ½.
63Fig 11.18
Figure shows B/C benefit to cost ratio of
investing in the current offspring. Benefit is
measured in benefit to current offspring and cost
is measured in reduction in future offspring.
64Parent-offspring conflict
- In instances where parents produce only half
siblings we should expect weaning conflict to
last longer because the current offspring is les
closely related to future offspring. - This has been confirmed in various field studies.
65(No Transcript)
66Siblicide
- In many species there is intense conflict between
siblings for food that may result in younger
weaker chicks starving to death. - In other species regardless of food supplies
first hatched offspring routinely kill their
siblings.
67Siblicide
- For example, in Black Eagles the first hatched
chick hatches several days before its sibling.
When the younger chick hatches its older sibling
attacks and kills it.
68Siblicide
- In species such as Black Eagles siblicide is
obligate in that the younger offspring is always
killed. Black Eagles are only capable of rearing
one young. - The most likely explanation for the later hatched
young is that for the parents it serves as an
insurance offspring in case the first offspring
fails to hatch or develop.
69Siblicide
- In other species such as Cattle Egrets there is
intense conflict that establishes a clear
age-based hierarchy in the brood that determines
how food is divided among the brood members. - In cattle egrets, younger chicks usually starve,
but if it is a good food year they often fledge.
70Siblicide
- Siblicide is thus facultative in cattle egrets
because restraint by the older chicks in not
killing the younger siblings can be rewarded in
good years. - In Black Eagles there is no prospect of two young
being reared, so the older chick ensures its own
survival by eliminating its sibling.
71Siblicide
- Siblicide shows that relatedness does not
necessarily lead to altruistic behavior. For
Cattle Egrets and Black Eagles selfishness is
better because the costs of altruism are too high.
72Reciprocal Altruism
- Some animals occasionally behave altruistically
towards non-relatives. - Such behavior is adaptive if the recipient is
likely to return the favor in the future.
73Reciprocal altruism
- Reciprocal altruism most likely in social animals
where individuals interact repeatedly because
they are long-lived and form groups, and also
when individuals have good memories.
74Reciprocal altruism in Vampire bats
- E.g. Vampire Bats. Feed on blood and share
communal roosts. - Bats may starve if they fail to feed several
nights in a row. - However, bats who have fed successfully often
regurgitate blood meals for unsuccessful bats.
75Reciprocal altruism in Vampire bats
- Cost of sharing some blood is relatively low for
donor bat but very valuable for recipient. - Research shows that Vampire bats share with
relatives, but also share with individuals who
have shared with them previously and with whom
they usually share a roost.
76Association is measure of how frequently two
individuals associate socially.
Regurgitators regurgitate to individuals
they associate with regularly.