Title: Georgian Verbal Morphology: A Learners Perspective
1Georgian Verbal MorphologyA Learners
Perspective
- Olya Gurevich
- NTL Lunch Talk
- January 31, 2003
2Introduction
- The Georgian verb presents problems for
traditional morphological approaches - Many of these problems can be resolved in an
approach compatible with language acquisition
3Language Learners Perspective
- Analyzing whole words
- Paying attention to frequency and to paradigmatic
contrasts - Parallel processing
- Can this be done in NTL?
4Phonology
- br?gvna - To tear into pieces
- gvbr?gvni - You are tearing us into pieces
- prckvna To peel, to rip off
- gvprckvni You are ripping us off
5The organization of the Georgian verb
- preverbAGR1theme Vowelstemthematic suffix
tense/aspect AGR2 - Ex. xat-av you (sg.) draw X
- v- xat-av I draw X
- m-xat-av you (sg.) draw me
- m-xat-av-t you (pl.) draw me
- m-xat-av-di-t you (pl.) drew me
- da-v- xat-av I will draw X
6Challenges Presented by Data
- General problem the meaning of a word is not
straightforwardly composed from the meanings of
the morphemes. - The particular combination of parts determines
the meaning of the form, in contrast with other
possible combinations. - This general property is manifested in several
ways
7Challenges (cont.)
- Agreement slots are not consistently associated
with argument properties (slot competition). - Agreement markers do not have a one-to-one
association with morphosyntactic properties (the
status of t).
8A sample paradigm
Forms of the verb to draw in the Present
9Problem 1 Slot Competition
- AGR1 Morphemes
- m 1SG Object
- gv 1PL Object
- g 2SG or 2PL Object
- v 1SG or 1PL Subject
- The AGR1 slot does not consistently indicate
properties of the same argument - But the morphemes correspond to particular
morphosyntactic properties
10Problem 2 The meaning of -t
- AGR2 morphemes
- en 3PL Subject
- s 3SG Subject except when 2PL Object
- t 1PL Subject, 2PL Subject, 1SG or 2SG Subject
with 2PL Object. - The morpheme t does not have a unique association
with a morphosyntactic property of an argument
11More on Slot Competition
- Even if we assign a meaning to each morpheme,
that meaning does not predict which morpheme will
show up in a form. - E.g. v vs. g, t vs. s.
12Problem 3 Inversion
- The agreement markers do not always correspond to
the same syntactic arguments. - In some tenses, m-, gv-, g- mark properties of
the subject, and v- marks properties of the
object. - There is a complete reversal of agreement markers
13Inversion (cont.)
- Ex. present tense (non-inverted)
- me en g-xatav en me m-xatav
- I you.SG draw you.SG me draw
- I am drawing you. You are drawing me.
- Perfect / evidential tense (inverted)
- me en mo-m-i-xatav-xar en me
mo-g-i-xatav-var - I you.SG has-drawn you.SG me
has-drawn - I have (apparently) drawn you. You have
(apparently) drawn me
14Inversion (cont.)
- Problem agreement markers cannot be uniquely
associated with properties of syntactic
arguments. - More on semantics of inversion later?
15Problem 4 Differences between tenses
- There is no unique morpheme that indicates the
tense of the verb. - Ex.
- v-xat-av I draw present
- da-v-xat-av I will draw future
- da-v-xat-e I drew aorist (perfective past)
- da-xat-a He drew aorist
- v-xat-av-di I was drawing past continuous
- da-v-xat-av-di I would draw past
frequentative
16General problem
- Many morphemes do not uniquely determine a
morpho-syntactic property. The properties of a
given form are determined by the combination of
morphemes used, in contrast to other possible
combinations.
17Why is this a problem for traditional approaches?
- Morphemic approaches morphemes contribute
individual meanings the whole is composed of the
parts - Rule-based approaches the addition of a morpheme
consumes or realizes part of the meaning.
Again, a unique association between an element of
form and an element of meaning.
18How can we avoid these problems?
- Analyze entire words and relationships between
words (i.e. paradigm structure) - Associate an entire word (i.e. a collection of
form elements) with a collection of meaning
elements. - Produce and recognize words using example
paradigms rather than specific rules.
19More evidence for a word-based approach
- Inflectional classes used to predict variable
selection of inflectional endings. - In Georgian, there are many variations for the
selection of endings - We would end up with thousands of inflectional
classes if it was the only relevant parameter
20Predicting a paradigm (cont.)
- Ex. Formation of the aorist
- 1st and 2nd person endings - 2 possibilities e,
i - 3rd person endings 2 possibilities a, o
- Thematic suffix 3 possibilities same as in
present, drop entirely, change - Stem changes 3 possibilities stay the same,
add the vowel e or a - Most, if not all, combinations of the above are
possible. If a single parameter had to account
for everything, there would be MANY inflectional
classes. - Add even more classes to account for forms in
other tenses.
21Predicting a paradigm (cont.)
- The combination of the following parameters
uniquely determines all forms of a verb - Thematic suffix - always there in the
Present-tense form - Shape of the root (whether or not it has a vowel,
which consonant it ends in) - can be seen in the
Present form - The pre-verb always there in Future or Aorist /
Imperative forms - Stem suppletion if occurs at all, it occurs in
the Future - Other irregularities are predictable from the
Infinitive or the Aorist (Imperative) forms - The meaning of the verb and its syntactic valence
22Predicting a paradigm (cont.)
- For predictive value, it does not matter which
Present or Future forms are used (i.e. 1st, 2nd,
or 3rd person). - In language learning, the most frequent forms are
used (Bybee 2001). - The forms needed to predict a paradigm
- Present
- Future
- Aorist (Imperative)
- These tenses are among the first learned by
children and the most frequent in discourse
23Predicting a paradigm (cont.)
- Ex to draw
- Present tense (2SG) xatav
- Stem xat has a vowel
- Thematic Suffix av
- Future tense (2SG) daxatav
- Preverb da
- No stem suppletion
- Imperative (2SG Aorist) daxate
- Aorist ending e
- Predicted choices
- Formation of 2nd subjunctive (TS drops, ending
o) - Formation of 1st and 2nd Resultant (TS stays,
ending s) - 3rd person aorist ending a
- No stem changes in any forms
24How does this work?
- Example paradigms (i.e. paradigms of the most
common verbs) are stored - For less frequent verbs, only the most frequent
(predictive) forms are stored - The relevant parameters in a form to be
recognized activate similar example paradigms,
possibly more than one - The example paradigm that fits the pattern best,
wins. The form is interpreted / constructed
based on that example. - Best fit is defined by a combination of
similarity and frequency of the example (Bybees
lexical strength).
25Can this be done in NTL?
- Neural nets are very good at associating
combinations of forms with combinations of
properties (distributed representations) - Neural nets are very good at finding best fit
solutions - Can a simple PDP net learn paradigm structure?
Seems like it should be able to.
26What about ECG?
- ECG cannot express paradigm structure, and refer
to other forms possible within a paradigm. - However, it should be able to send off a form
to the PDP net and get back a combination of
morphosyntactic properties. This process could
be reversed for production, as well a
combination of properties is run through a PDP
net which gives a likely form - Any reason why this wouldnt work? Any other
ideas? - Concern how do you train a PDP net to produce
example paradigms that are compatible with actual
human input?
27So Georgian is weird. Who cares?
- The difficulty of assigning meaning to morphemes
(or even words) is not unique to Georgian. - Periphrasis (e.g. the English auxiliaries) works
this way too, as well inflectional morphology of
many other languages. - Perhaps this could be a general solution to those
kinds of problems - Mappings between form and meaning can be
many-to-many, and it is necessary to know what
the possibilities are to know what a particular
combination of form elements means.
28Semantics of Inversion
- Inversion in Transitive and other Active Verbs
- In Present tense, Subject is in Nominative, Obj
is in DAT/ACC - In Evidential tenses, Subj is in DAT, Obj is in
NOM - The reversal of agreement markers corresponds to
a reversal of case markings on the arguments. - The subject in Inverted tenses is less active
(less responsible) and is therefore indicated by
a less prominent case
29Inversion (cont.)
- Present
- k'ac-i dzaql-s xatav-s
- man.NOM dog-sg.DAT draw.PRES
- 'The man draws / is drawing the dog
- Evidential
- k'ac-s dzaqli da-u-xatav-s
- man.DAT he.NOM draw.1RESULT
- 'The man has (apparently) drawn the dog'
30Inversion (cont.)
- Present
- k'ac-i me m-xatav-s
- man.NOM me draw.PRES
- 'The man draws / is drawing me
- Evidential
- k'ac-s me da-v-u-xatav-var
- man.DAT me draw.1RESULT
- 'The man has (apparently) drawn me'
31Inversion (cont.)
- There may be a separation between semantic roles
(most prominent argument, least prominent
argument etc.) and syntactic arguments (subject,
object). - In non-inverted tenses, subject is most active /
most prominent - In inverted tenses, the mappings are reversed
- Morphology is sensitive to semantic roles rather
than syntactic arguments - m-, gv- markers mark the less prominent argument,
v- marks the more prominent argument)
32Inversion (cont.)
- Indirect Verbs
- Verbs like have, want always have inverted
markers, their subjects are always in Dative - Subjects of these verbs are inherently
non-active, receiving the action - These verbs are among the first learned by
children and present no problems - Regular inversion is learned late and is
difficult for children to master - Indirect verbs must have a different
representation they are high-frequency irregular
verbs