Title: Six to Nine Months
1Six to Nine Months
Created by Ilse DeKoeyer-Laros, Ph.D.
2Overview Chapter 7
- Physical and Motor Development
- Perceptual Development
- Cognitive Development
- Emotional Development
- Social and Language Development
- Family and Society
Experiential Exercises Co-regulating with Baby
Experiential Exercises Co-regulating with
Baby
3Introduction
- Between 6 and 9 months,
babies grow more adventurous - physical they start to creep
or
crawl on their own - psychological they begin to take initiative and
call attention
to themselves - Infants now develop a serious interest in the
object world, and come to understand that objects
are whole entities with an existence separate
from their own
Picture from http//flickr.com/photos/erikrasmuss
en/2511777779/
4Physical and Motor Development
- Between 6 and 9 months, infants develop
- independent sitting
- supported standing
- rolling over
- creeping or crawling
- By 9 months, infants can
- take a few steps while holding on to furniture or
an adult - pick up small objects using just the tips of the
thumb and index finger
Picture from http//www.imaginarybinky.com/2008/0
4/let-sun-shine-down.html
5Physical and Motor DevelopmentHand Movements and
Hand Preference
Functions of the hemispheres of the brain
- right hemisphere spatial patterns
nonlinguistic (e.g., emotional) information
processing - the left hemisphere sequential processing of the
sort used in understanding language
- linked to handedness the preference for the use
of one hand over another
Picture from www.morphonix.com/.../specimens/hemispheres.html
6Physical and Motor Development Hand Movements
and Hand Preference
- Infants begin to show hand preference around 2
months, when visually guided reaching begins - More permanent hand preferences do not emerge
until the 2nd year
- 30-50 of infants under age 1 show a right-hand
preference when reaching this preference is
relatively stable over the 1st year - 10-30 have a left-hand preference in reaching
Picture from path31.blogspot.com/2007_09_01_archi
ve.html
7Physical and Motor Development Hand Movements
and Hand Preference
- Around 6 months, infants
- begin reaching with a single hand
- learn to sit without support extend the
non-reaching hand backward to balance their upper
bodies - two-handed reaches become more sophisticated
- with larger objects (like a big ball)
- cross the mid-line of the body
8Physical and Motor DevelopmentCrawling
- Being able to extend one arm independently of the
other is believed to be important for the
development of crawling (see Table 7.2) - while babies are still reaching with two hands at
the same time, they either creep or rock - infants begin to crawl when they can reach with
one hand
Picture from http//www.sover.net/sweeneyc/babyg
irlold.html
9Physical and Motor DevelopmentCrawling
- Not all infants go through this sequence
- infants who creep before they crawl are better at
crawling they move faster and their movements
are larger and more efficient - non-creepers become proficient crawlers after a
couple of weeks
Picture from private collection
10Physical and Motor Development How Motor Skills
Develop
- Dynamic systems theory new motor skills develop
by adding additional components to existing
skills - crawling even when infants can get on hands
knees, they cannot crawl because they cant
alternate extension of the arms and legs - walking 9-month-olds can pull themselves to
standing, take steps while holding onto
something, and alternate leg movements but they
cant walk, because they lack the capacity to
balance
11Physical and Motor Development How Motor Skills
Develop
- The moving room recreates the visual experience
of moving without taking steps at the same time - infants under 1 year will fall in the direction
in which the wall appears to be moving - infants older than 1 year may sway but are less
likely to lose their balance
12Physical and Motor DevelopmentHow Motor Skills
Develop
- Motor development is a complex systems
interaction of - the different parts of the motor system (legs,
trunk, arms) - the perceptual system
- the environment in which the child is moving
- For example,
- infants can make walking movements if they are
supported by an infant walker or an adult
Picture from private collection
13Perceptual DevelopmentRecognition of Objects
Depth
- Under 6 months
- object recognition and depth perception are
easier if the objects are moving and if real
objects are presented - After 6 months, infants can
- infer object properties depth from visual cues
alone - see three dimensions when they are shown
objects in two dimensions (e.g., in a drawing)
14Perceptual DevelopmentRecognition of Objects
Depth
- By 7 months, infants use visual cues to judge
depth distance - infants with a patch over one eye will reach
toward the larger of two identical pictures of a
face, apparently perceiving it as closer - infants ability to recognize objects in two
dimensions leads to increased interest in picture
books and television at this age
15Perceptual DevelopmentRecognition of Objects and
Depth
- Haptic perception perception of the properties
of an object using touch - newborns can distinguish different properties of
objects by using their mouths - between 4 and 6 months, infants explore objects
actively, combining hand, mouth, and vision - after 6 months, infants develop specialized hand
movements to detect information about specific
object properties such as size, texture, and
shape
16Perceptual DevelopmentOther Perceptual
Developments
- By 6 months, babies
- recognize differences between simple melodies
- can use cross-modal perception to infer
information about object properties - infants who are familiarized with an object only
by touch can recognize the object by sight alone - if babies hear a sound in the dark, they will
reach for an object in the direction of the sound
17Perceptual DevelopmentOther Perceptual
Developments
- In short, 6- to 9-month-olds use subtle cues to
infer regularities in their perceptual world - they can learn from pictures
in books
and on television - they pick up relationships
between
different senses
to pay attention
to things that
interest them most - These perceptual abilities lead to clear
preferences (e.g., for particular pictures,
objects, and tastes)
Picture from flickr.com/photos/offwithyourhead/80
276937/
18Cognitive DevelopmentMemory
- By 7 months,
- infants can remember how to make a mobile move
for as long as 21 days, without a reminder - memories are less context dependent
- infants can remember a salient event that has
been learned in different (but related)
situations - infants can remember longer sequences of events,
like longer melodies - However, memory is still tied to the situation
19Cognitive DevelopmentInformation Processing
- At 7 months, infants are able to group stimuli
into higher-order conceptual categories - 7-month-olds (but not 5-month-olds) recognized
the same faces shown in different positions - they distinguished smiling from non-smiling faces
- they recognize a prototype from distorted
versions - they distinguish horses from other four-legged
mammals
20Cognitive Development Information Processing
- Infants of this age also
- understand that moving objects should follow
along their prior path of movement larger
objects can support smaller objects - respond differentially when the same object is
placed above or below another object, showing
that they have a category for these spatial
relationships - seem to have a concept of number
- they dishabituated when a puppets jumps changed,
from two to three or from three to two
21Cognitive DevelopmentSecondary Circular Reactions
- Sensorimotor Stage III (4 to 8 or 9 months)
Secondary circular reactions - infants begin to repeat actions that, by chance,
produce some effect on the objects and people in
the environment - once the chance discovery is made, infants make
deliberate, intentional attempts to repeat that
action
22Cognitive DevelopmentSecondary Circular Reactions
- Infants also vary the actions in order to explore
changes in the effect - they will drop objects off the edge of their high
chairs - they shake objects in different ways to notice
the effect or repeatedly dump things out of
containers - Repeated occurrences in the environment take on
meaning for the baby (see Observation 7.2) - by 7 months, Laurent knew that he would be fed
shortly after he heard his mothers bed creak
23Cognitive DevelopmentSecondary Circular Reactions
- Infants are becoming more goal directed can
perceive others intentional behavior - in one study, 9-month-old infants looked longer
when a grasping hand contacted a toy than when
the toy was touched with the back of a hand
Picture from flickr.com/photos/36908756_at_N00/30147
4833
24Cognitive DevelopmentOut of Sight, Out of Mind?
- Object permanence the ability to remain aware
of an object even after it has gone out of sight - infants will not actively search for an object
that has been hidden until after 9 months
Picture from http//scienceblogs.com/cognitivedai
ly/2005/08/do_babies_know_if_hidden_objec.php
25Cognitive DevelopmentOut of Sight, Out of Mind?
- In one study, 7- to 8-month-olds saw an object
disappear behind one of two screens - a hand reached behind the screens and reappeared
holding the object in either a possible situation
or an impossible situation - Infants looked longer at the hand following the
impossible situation compared to the possible one - In a similar study, infants not only looked
longer at the impossible situation, they also
looked more at their parents as if to share their
puzzlement
26Cognitive DevelopmentOut of Sight, Out of Mind?
- Infants of this age are becoming aware of objects
and people as whole entities - people, as opposed to objects, are understood by
infants as having intentions - the ability to perceive anothers intentions
corresponds with infants awareness of their own
intentions, their ability to have an effect on
the environment
27Emotional DevelopmentNegative Emotions
- Anger arises when infants cannot succeed at being
an effective causal agent - accompanied by crying, but facial expression and
underlying feeling are different from distress - In one study
- 2- and 4-month-olds reacted to inoculations with
physical distress, crying with tightly shut eyes - 7-month-olds responded with more angry
expressions, crying with open, vigilant eyes
28Emotional DevelopmentNegative Emotions
- Anger can be adaptive and useful
- In one study, infants were taught to pull a
string to activate a slide projection music - After this, the experimenters stopped turning on
the slide projector and music when the infant
pulled - Most infants reacted with anger, but some showed
sadness - the infants who expressed anger immediately
became interested again when the contingency was
renewed - those who showed sadness reacted with less
enjoyment
29Emotional DevelopmentNegative Emotions
- Expressions of anger are also seen in
7-month-olds when they are frustrated - e.g., when a teething biscuit is removed from
their mouths or when their arms are restrained - Separation distress
- after 6 months, infants respond to parental
separation with some anger, especially if the
parent happens to be a part of the infants
activity (e.g., play) when he or she leaves
Picture from http//meidays.blogdrive.com/
30Emotional DevelopmentNegative Emotions
- Wariness
- infants may become quiet and stare at a stranger
or a strange situation, knit their brows, become
momentarily sober, and look away - wariness allows the infant to observe what is
happening is a more adaptive reaction to
strange situations than the withdrawal of
infantile fussing and crying
31Emotional DevelopmentPositive Emotions
- Positive emotions become more complex
- Different types of smiles had different meanings
depending on whether the infant gazed at the
mother or not - Simple smiling gazing at mother during
peekaboo - enjoyment of recognition or of readiness to
engage in play - Simple smiles without gazing at mother after a
tickle - often accompanied by gasping for air and sighing
perhaps associated with enjoyment of relief or
of relaxation
32Emotional DevelopmentPositive Emotions
- Duchenne smiles occur with gazing at mother when
she uncovers her face during peekaboo - may reflect an enjoyment of agency, sensing
oneself as an active rather than passive
participant in the game - Duchenne smiles without gazing at mother occur
most during a tickle, often as infants turn their
bodies away as if trying to hide or protect
themselves - may reflect an enjoyment of hiding or perhaps an
enjoyment of escape
Picture from http//www.associatedcontent.com/ima
ge/123286/index.html?cat25
33Emotional DevelopmentPositive Emotions
- There is a growing ability to communicate with
others about emotions - around 8 months, infants who smile when looking
at an object will turn to smile at a nearby adult
- By 6 months, babies will laugh
- at jokes
- at very abrupt and highly arousing stimuli
- at things that once made them cry, such as a loud
noise or a loss of balance - in one study, they cried when a stranger wore a
mask, but laughed when their mothers did
34Emotional DevelopmentEmotion Regulation
- Infants are beginning to use cognition to decide
what to feel, a process known as appraisal - there is a growing relationship between infant
emotion attention to emotion-related events and
processes
Picture from www.spicetart.com/growing_ivy/page/2
/
35Emotional DevelopmentEmotion Regulation
- Gender differences
- In one study, 6-month-old boys and girls were
observed during face-to-face play, followed by
maternal still-face - Boys were more likely than girls to
- smile vocalize as well as show anger or
distress during the still-face - have a more positive interaction with the mother
during the normal face-to-face period - Girls gazed more at objects showed more
interest
36Emotional DevelopmentRecognition of Emotional
Expressions
- Between 6 and 9 months
- babies seem more capable of recognizing smiles
than other expressions - their ability to distinguish between other
expressions, such as fear and anger, is
relatively poor - Individual differences
- 7-month-old infants whose mothers show a lot of
positive emotions are more likely to respond to
negative facial expressions, perhaps because of
their relative novelty
37Emotional DevelopmentRecognition of Emotional
Expressions
- 7-month-olds ability to distinguish between
emotions improves when - facial expressions are combined with voices
expressing the same emotions - faces are presented dynamically
- They also recognize whether a facial expression
is paired with a matching vs. a mismatched
intonation - e.g., when an angry expression is matched with an
angry vs. happy tone of voice
38Emotional DevelopmentRecognition of Emotional
Expressions
- Infants of this age prefer to look at faces
judged by adults to be attractive - apparently, attractiveness, like recognition of
particular people, can be inferred from more
global features of the face that do not involve
specific expressions - Infants can also distinguish between the faces of
children and adults
39Emotional DevelopmentInfant Temperament
- Temperament
- a persistent pattern of emotion and emotion
regulation in the infants relationship to people
and things in the environment - Some aspects of temperament are partly inherited
- negativity and inhibition appear early in life
and are persistent in 5-10 of \ infants up until
5 to 7 years - similar proportions of persistently inhibited
children are found in different countries and
even in infant monkeys
40Emotional DevelopmentInfant Temperament
- Infants who were the most inhibited
- were more likely to be subdued in unfamiliar
situations, have a dour mood, report anxiety, and
have an overactive sympathetic nervous system
response as teenagers - showed a higher activation in the amygdala (part
of the limbic system responsive to fear) when
viewing pictures of unfamiliar faces as adults
41Emotional DevelopmentInfant Temperament
- Infants and children who have difficulties with
attention and emotion regulation (rated as highly
reactive, emotional, inattentive, or inhibited)
have different patterns of activity in the
prefrontal cortex compared to well-regulated
infants - For example, inhibition is related to brain wave
and heart rate patterns as well as to stress
responses to frustration - Stress responses to frustration such as heart
rate acceleration, cortisol secretion, and
sympathetic nervous system activation are
present at an early age for some inhibited
infants and may persist for periods of up to 1
year
42Emotional DevelopmentInfant Temperament
- Role of parents
- infants who are more inhibited are more likely to
have parents who are introverted anxious - infant inhibition negativity are related to
lower scores on maternal adaptation to pregnancy,
sensitivity to the infant after birth, and
self-esteem - mothers who rate infant cries as more aversive
are more likely to rate their infants as
difficult - These findings do not rule out a genetic
explanations
43Emotional DevelopmentInfant Temperament
- A finding that may call the genetic explanation
into question is that children do not necessarily
exhibit continuity of temperament - inhibited children may, with sensitive child
rearing, eventually lose their extreme
sensitivity - normal children may become more inhibited in
extremely stressful environments
44Emotional DevelopmentInfant Temperament
- Continuity discontinuity
- extreme fussiness at birth predicts later
emotionality in full-term infants, but not in
premature infants - temperament most likely does not contribute to
long-term cognitive deficit or enhancement - parental behavior may attenuate the long-term
effects of early temperamental characteristic - inhibited children who showed more positive
emotion were less likely to be inhibited at age 3
than inhibited children who tended to be more
negative
45Emotional DevelopmentInfant Temperament
- Parental and child factors can interact to
influence the stability of temperament over time - Temperament assessed at the end of the infancy
period, between 2 and 4 years of age, tends to
show long-term stability - 2-year-olds who were rated as more difficult had
more attention problems and aggressive behavior
at 12 years - 3-year-olds who were rated low on self-control
had more adjustment problems and interpersonal
conflicts as adults
46Emotional DevelopmentThe Measurement of
Temperament
- Temperament is difficult to measure in a reliable
and valid manner - some researches have conducted observations
- usually, parents are asked to rate their childs
temperaments - However, when mothers and fathers are asked to
rate the same child, their reports agree only
about half the time - there is more agreement about the difficulty of
an infant than about any other dimension of
temperament
47Emotional DevelopmentThe Measurement of
Temperament
- The correlation between parental reports and
behavior observed in a laboratory improves if
infants behaviors are extreme - Explanations of differences between parent
reports and observed behavior include - infants behave differently in different
situations - questions on rating scales dont reflect childs
individuality - parental reports reflect parents personalities
or psychological state - for instance, multiparous extroverted mothers
were more likely to rate their infants as easy
48Emotional DevelopmentThe Measurement of
Temperament
- The best research strategy
- a combination of parental reports, direct
observations, and physiological measures (such as
cortisol and sympathetic nervous system activity)
made at repeated intervals in the childs life
Picture from http//ethiopia.adoptionblogs.com/we
blogs/african-american-dolls
49Social and Language DevelopmentSocial-Object
Frames
- As infants become increasingly interested in
objects, between 4 and 6 months of age, earlier
face-to-face frames give way to social-object
play frames - the infants developmental task is to integrate
interest in objects with the desire to remain
socially and emotionally connected to the parents - At first, infants are primarily focused on the
objects - it is up to the parents to provide frames for
mutual communication about the objects -
50Social and Language DevelopmentSocial-Object
Frames
- Coordination of attention to people objects is
enhanced if parents regularly create
object-directed frames - the more attentive animated parents are, the
more likely that the infant will learn to
co-regulate attention with others - infants who are more attentive to what adults do
and say are more likely to learn language and to
learn, by age 3 or 4 years, to share the mental
perspectives of other people
Picture from private collection
51Social and Language DevelopmentSocial-Object
Frames
- At 6 months, infants appear to be in a receptive
mode, ready to participate in the frames created
by the parents
- By 8 months, they are beginning to take
initiatives in social frames - they start making jokes
- they ask to be picked up by making sad facial
expressions or raising their hands above their
heads - they smile and laugh more toward familiar and
trusted adults than toward unfamiliar ones
Picture from private collection
52Social and Language DevelopmentBabbling
- Babbling begins after 6 months
- sounds as though babies are talking to themselves
as they roll off a string of related vowel and
consonant sounds to accompany their eating or
playing - has the intonation contours (the rising and
falling pitches) of sentences - the intonation contours of babbling match the
intonation contours of the speech spoken in the
infants home (e.g., French, Chinese, or Arabic)
53Social and Language DevelopmentBabbling
- In one study, some mothers were asked to respond
contingently to infant babbling, while another
group was asked not to respond to the babbling - infants whose mothers were contingent produced
more mature babbles that had more recognizable
syllables, strong contrasts between consonants
and vowels, and a more fully voiced sound - this suggests that babbling may be speech-like
because it occurs during parent-infant contingent
vocal interaction.
54Social and Language DevelopmentBabbling
- Right-handed reaching and rhythmical banging
increase at the same age infants begin to babble - the right hand is controlled by the left brain,
known to be the primary location of speech
processing - vocalizations come increasingly under the control
of the left brain, setting the stage for linking
vocalization and cognition, necessary for the
development of speech - Babbling is more likely to be accompanied by
(right-) hand arm movements than leg movements
55Social and Language DevelopmentSpeech Perception
- Before 6 months, infants can distinguish sound
contrasts from many different languages - they start to lose this ability between 6-9
months - In one developmental study
- younger infants (4-6 months) could distinguish
the syllable contrasts from all three languages
tested (English, Salish, and Hindi) - older infants (10-12 months) could only
distinguish between the contrasts of the language
heard in the home
56Social and Language DevelopmentSpeech Perception
- The loss of perceptual sensitivity may be related
to the selective processes of brain development - at first, synapses are overproduced
- later some are selected and strengthened
synapses for sounds that are not frequently heard
disappear - By 9 months, American infants prefer to listen to
words having a strong-weak stress pattern - 6-month-olds showed no such preference
57Social and Language DevelopmentSpeech Perception
- By the second half of the first year, infants
begin to recognize produce some of the
characteristics of language as a system of sounds
- However, babbling infants are not trying to talk
- they are exploring how to make familiar sounds,
rather than as trying to communicate with sounds - it seems as if they first learn the music and
then the words - this music is learned in the context of
parent-infant frames, including social games
58Social and Language DevelopmentParent-Infant
Games
- By 8 months, new social frames emerge in the
parent-infant relationship - infants take more initiative
- As infants get older, they learn to play new
social games, such as point and name and give
and take at 12 months - games like gonna get you and horsie, in which
the 6-month-old played a relatively passive role,
occur only rarely at 12 months
59Social and Language DevelopmentCultural
Differences
- Climate is one factor that accounts for cultural
differences in child-rearing patterns
- in warm countries, infants tend to be carried,
remain in close physical contact, and to be
breast-fed longer than infants reared in cold
climates - infants in cold climates are more likely to be
separated from their mothers at an earlier age
Picture from saindonienne.wordpress.com/
60Social and Language DevelopmentCultural
Differences
- Cultural differences in parental beliefs about
emotion regulation and communication about
emotions - in North America Korea, parents were concerned
about stimulation to foster development - Italian mothers had similar feelings about love
and emotional closeness as North American mothers
but were less focused on cognitive stimulation - Similar patterns are found in Latin-American
cultures
61Social and Language DevelopmentCultural
Differences
- In a study of physical contact during play
between Hispanic- and Anglo-American mothers and
their 9-month-old infants - the overall amount of physical contact did not
differ - Hispanic mothers touched, kissed, hugged, and
held their infants physically closer than Anglo
mothers - on questionnaires, the Hispanic mothers reported
placing a higher value on touch and affection
than Anglo mothers
Picture from www.momease.com
62Social and Language DevelopmentCultural
Differences
- In technical and industrial societies, including
Japan, Korea, Europe, North America, and urban
families everywhere - parents will begin to interpret the infants
intentions - the next step is to help the infant carry out the
intended act - parents often create new intentions that were not
there in the first place - In nontechnical agricultural and hunter-gatherer
communities, adults are more directive and
ritualistic - Chomorro mothers (from the Pacific island of
Guam), were highly directive repetitive when
interacting with infants
63Social and Language DevelopmentCultural
Differences
- Each style has evolved to fit the needs of the
particular culture problems may arise, however,
when cultures are forced to interact - Hispanic Americans, as a minority culture in the
United States, often feel self-conscious in the
company of their Anglo-American neighbors and
think of themselves as too affectionate with
their infants - According to ecological systems theory, this is a
conflict between the microsystem of the family
and the macrosystem of the culture
64Social and Language DevelopmentSelf-Awareness
- Between 6 and 9 months, babies call attention to
themselves in ways that did not exist previously - These features make up a sense of a
differentiated ecological self - asking for help
- taking initiative
- clowning and showing off
- demanding
- hiding and escaping
Picture from private collection
65Social and Language Development Self-Awareness
- At this age
- emotions become more complex
- infants begin to take initiative
- infants begin to seem to have their own
personalities - But they do not have a sense of subjectivity
- they have feelingsgetting angry or happybut
cannot yet stand apart from those feelings - they do not have a sense of an I that feels,
and, consequently, they do not have a sense that
other people are separate subjects with their own
feelings
66Family and SocietyMaternal Employment
- In 2001,
- 64 of U.S. mothers with children under the age
of 6 were in the work force - mothers also do a substantial amount of unpaid
work (e.g., child care, household work),
estimated to be worth about 27,000 per woman per
year
67Family and SocietyMaternal Employment
- In general, infant-mother attachment is not
seriously altered by maternal employment - If attachment is going to be affected, it is most
likely to decline between employed mothers and
their infant sons rather than their daughters - boys are perceived as more independent and as
requiring less parental nurture and attention
than girls, who are seen as more vulnerable - there is a correlation between a sons insecure
attachment and a mothers perceived level of
stress
68Family and SocietyMaternal Employment
- A number of studies find that the important
variable is the mothers desire to work - problems with coping, dissatisfaction with life,
depression, and loneliness are significantly
higher in young mothers who do not work outside
the home than in those who do - there are higher levels of functioning in
families in which the mothers are employed
69Family and SocietyMaternal Employment
- Whether mothers work by choice or necessity, they
typically end the day fatigued because role
overload - the demands of a role are more than an individual
can easily cope with or when the same person is
required to perform too many roles - Role overload increases if the child has a
difficult (age 1) or hostile aggressive (age 3)
temperament - in this case, mothers a more likely to perceive
themselves as less competent in both the
parenting and work roles, and are more likely to
feel depressed
70Family and SocietyMaternal Employment
- When women work, fathers can also experience a
form of role overload - such fathers show more negative behavior with
their infants during the first year - after the first year, they are just as sensitive
to their infants as other husbands - When women remain at home, fathers can choose
when and how to become involved with their
infants - these fathers show more positive emotion toward
their infants and are more attuned to the
infants needs, especially if the infants are
boys
71Family and SocietyParental Leave Policies
- Even when mothers are paid, they earn only 71
cents for every dollar earned by a man in the
same position - Some alternatives exist, but they are not
widespread - more flexible work schedules (flextime) began to
be instituted in Europe in the early 1960s - Swedish workers are entitled to maternity and
paternity leaves - Swedish women earn about 90 the wages men do for
similar jobs 86 of women with young children
are in the workforce
72Family and SocietyParental Leave Policies
- In 1993, the U.S. Congress enacted the Family and
Medical Leave Act of 1993 (FMLA, Public Law
103-3), which established a family leave policy - provides unpaid leave from employment for up to
12 weeks without loss of rank or position in the
workplace in businesses with more than 50
employees - applies to both mothers and fathers as well as to
non-pregnancy-related illnesses - unfortunately, 95 of businesses are exempt from
the FMLA because they have fewer than 50
employees
73Family and SocietyParental Leave Policies
- Mothers are more likely to take a parental leave
- on average, about 3 months
- mothers who take shorter leaves are more likely
to feel stress symptoms of depression, show
negative emotions toward their infants and
spouses, and to have less interest in their
infants - The average length of leave for fathers was 6.5
days, with 71 of fathers taking 5 or fewer days - fathers who took shorter leaves worked for
employers who were did not have a positive
reaction to the employees fatherhood, were less
involved with their infants, and had less
communication with their spouse about the infant
74Family and SocietyParental Leave Policies
- Compared to other industrialized countries, the
United States is not a nation that fully supports
children and families - With little opportunity to take time off from
work - mothers cannot breast feed for as long as
recommended by the American Academy of Pediatrics
(for 12 months) - mothers may choose drug-assisted childbirth or
C-sections, even if they would have preferred a
natural birth, to get back to work sooner
75Family and SocietyNurturance Toward Infants
- Interest and ability to care for babies
- is present in young boys and girls
- will continue in both if fostered by the
environment - In one study,
- girls approached a baby more than did boys
- once the children were near the baby, both boys
and girls spoke to, reached out for, and touched
the baby equally - In another study,
- boys and girls approached babies equally
- at 2 and 3 years, boys were more likely to
approach male babies, girls were more likely to
approach female babies
76Family and Society Nurturance Toward Infants
- Preschoolers speech to babies
- both boy and girl preschoolers modify their
speech to babies to make it sound more like
motherese - most preschoolers rarely asked questions of the
baby (question asking is a major form of adult
speech to infants) - 25 of preschoolers used endearing terms toward
the baby and asked soliciting questions (Are you
hungry? Are you getting frustrated?)
77Family and Society Nurturance Toward Infants
- Girls aged 8 to 14 interact more with babies and
ignore them less than boys do - boys and girls are equally physiologically
aroused or unaroused by the sight of an infant - differences in male vs. female interest in babies
continue through high school but seem to vanish
for college students young adults - Parents vs. nonparents
- the most responsive group is usually new mothers
- mens child-rearing status does not affect their
responsiveness to babies
78Family and Society Parenthood Mothers versus
Fathers
- Mothers tend to be more accurate in identifying
the type of cry (pain, distress, etc.) than
fathers - Both mothers and fathers can distinguish their
own infants cry from cries of unfamiliar infants
Picture from http//www.julienna.com/pictures/Dad
20and20Baby20on20Slide.jpg
79Family and SocietyParenthood Mothers versus
Fathers
- Father-infant interaction
- used to be less contingently responsive than
mothers but no differences were found in recent
studies - play and interaction are more directive show
abrupt changes of activity - cultural differences in the amount of father
involvement - Mother-infant interaction
- games are quieter and depend more on the pace set
by the infant - engage in more caregiving
80Family and SocietyGrandparents
- One study found that grandparents of infants were
more responsive to babies than were parents of
adolescents or of grown children who had left
home - grandmothers were more responsive than
grandfathers - grandfathers were more responsive than men at
other ages - Grandmother-infant attachment at 1 year
- when grandmothers spend much time with the baby,
mothers grandmothers are nearly interchangeable
as attachment figures - the more time a grandmother spends with the baby,
the more secure the attachment relationship
81Family and SocietyGrandparents
- In the 1990s, mothers and grandmothers were
generally in agreement over beliefs - mothers were more accepting of messiness and
nudity indoors, more relaxed about when to begin
toilet training, and less rigid in
differentiating sex roles in child play - Some studies have shown that African-American
grandmothers are more involved with their infant
grandchildren than Caucasian-Americans - extended family is important in the reduction of
family stress, esp. for low-income, teenage, and
single mothers
82Experiential ExercisesRolling Over
- This exercise is about the connection between the
core and its influence on an infants movements - Lying on your back, place an object on the floor
directly above your head. - Turn your head to the right and try to look at
your object, so that your back begins to arch.
Relax for 30 seconds, then repeat. - You may notice that your back is arching so much
so that you end up on your side. Once this
happens, try and balance yourself like a see-saw. - Once you are balanced, relax your core see if
you fall to one side or the other - After the roll, you may notice that one arm is
trapped underneath you. Flex your core so as to
create a space between your chest and the floor
to allow you to free your arm. - Now look at your object. Can you reach it?
83Experiential ExercisesBeginning to Crawl
- Crouched on your hands and knees on the floor,
slowly, raise one knee, hold it for a few
seconds, and bring it back down. - Notice how your weight shifts when you do this
motion. - Again, very slowly, raise your other knee and
repeat the movement. Do this also with both of
your hands. - Bring your knees together so that your feet are
close enough to touch. Repeat the slow-motion
raising and lowering movements with both of your
legs. Notice how this feels - Now, spread your knees on the ground far apart.
Repeat the same slow-motion movements. Notice how
this feels. - Experiment with different spacing between your
knees. Where is the best balance between
instability and stability?
84Experiential ExercisesBeginning to Crawl
- Slowly lift your right hand and your right knee
at the same time. How is your weight shifted and
distributed? Do the same with your left hand and
knee. - Next try lifting opposite hands and knees. Is
this easier? - Rock back and forth on your hands, shifting your
weight to and from your heels. Does this feel
like getting ready to move? - Crawl a few steps forward. Notice the order of
limbs that you move. If you lift your hands and
knees diagonallym this is called contralateral
crawling. If you lift your hands and knees on the
same side, this is called homolateral crawling. - Try crawling a few steps backward. Is this easier
than going forward? Babies often crawl backwards
before they crawl forwards.