Six to Nine Months - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

About This Presentation
Title:

Six to Nine Months

Description:

Six to Nine Months Fogel Chapter 7 Created by Ilse DeKoeyer-Laros, Ph.D. – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:361
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 85
Provided by: Goo7294
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Six to Nine Months


1
Six to Nine Months
  • Fogel
  • Chapter 7

Created by Ilse DeKoeyer-Laros, Ph.D.
2
Overview Chapter 7
  • Physical and Motor Development
  • Perceptual Development
  • Cognitive Development
  • Emotional Development
  • Social and Language Development
  • Family and Society

Experiential Exercises Co-regulating with Baby
Experiential Exercises Co-regulating with
Baby
3
Introduction
  • Between 6 and 9 months,
    babies grow more adventurous
  • physical they start to creep
    or
    crawl on their own
  • psychological they begin to take initiative and
    call attention
    to themselves
  • Infants now develop a serious interest in the
    object world, and come to understand that objects
    are whole entities with an existence separate
    from their own

Picture from http//flickr.com/photos/erikrasmuss
en/2511777779/
4
Physical and Motor Development
  • Between 6 and 9 months, infants develop
  • independent sitting
  • supported standing
  • rolling over
  • creeping or crawling
  • By 9 months, infants can
  • take a few steps while holding on to furniture or
    an adult
  • pick up small objects using just the tips of the
    thumb and index finger

Picture from http//www.imaginarybinky.com/2008/0
4/let-sun-shine-down.html
5
Physical and Motor DevelopmentHand Movements and
Hand Preference
Functions of the hemispheres of the brain
  • right hemisphere spatial patterns
    nonlinguistic (e.g., emotional) information
    processing
  • the left hemisphere sequential processing of the
    sort used in understanding language
  • linked to handedness the preference for the use
    of one hand over another

Picture from www.morphonix.com/.../specimens/hemispheres.html
6
Physical and Motor Development Hand Movements
and Hand Preference
  • Infants begin to show hand preference around 2
    months, when visually guided reaching begins
  • More permanent hand preferences do not emerge
    until the 2nd year
  • 30-50 of infants under age 1 show a right-hand
    preference when reaching this preference is
    relatively stable over the 1st year
  • 10-30 have a left-hand preference in reaching

Picture from path31.blogspot.com/2007_09_01_archi
ve.html
7
Physical and Motor Development Hand Movements
and Hand Preference
  • Around 6 months, infants
  • begin reaching with a single hand
  • learn to sit without support extend the
    non-reaching hand backward to balance their upper
    bodies
  • two-handed reaches become more sophisticated
  • with larger objects (like a big ball)
  • cross the mid-line of the body

8
Physical and Motor DevelopmentCrawling
  • Being able to extend one arm independently of the
    other is believed to be important for the
    development of crawling (see Table 7.2)
  • while babies are still reaching with two hands at
    the same time, they either creep or rock
  • infants begin to crawl when they can reach with
    one hand

Picture from http//www.sover.net/sweeneyc/babyg
irlold.html
9
Physical and Motor DevelopmentCrawling
  • Not all infants go through this sequence
  • infants who creep before they crawl are better at
    crawling they move faster and their movements
    are larger and more efficient
  • non-creepers become proficient crawlers after a
    couple of weeks

Picture from private collection
10
Physical and Motor Development How Motor Skills
Develop
  • Dynamic systems theory new motor skills develop
    by adding additional components to existing
    skills
  • crawling even when infants can get on hands
    knees, they cannot crawl because they cant
    alternate extension of the arms and legs
  • walking 9-month-olds can pull themselves to
    standing, take steps while holding onto
    something, and alternate leg movements but they
    cant walk, because they lack the capacity to
    balance

11
Physical and Motor Development How Motor Skills
Develop
  • The moving room recreates the visual experience
    of moving without taking steps at the same time
  • infants under 1 year will fall in the direction
    in which the wall appears to be moving
  • infants older than 1 year may sway but are less
    likely to lose their balance

12
Physical and Motor DevelopmentHow Motor Skills
Develop
  • Motor development is a complex systems
    interaction of
  • the different parts of the motor system (legs,
    trunk, arms)
  • the perceptual system
  • the environment in which the child is moving
  • For example,
  • infants can make walking movements if they are
    supported by an infant walker or an adult

Picture from private collection
13
Perceptual DevelopmentRecognition of Objects
Depth
  • Under 6 months
  • object recognition and depth perception are
    easier if the objects are moving and if real
    objects are presented
  • After 6 months, infants can
  • infer object properties depth from visual cues
    alone
  • see three dimensions when they are shown
    objects in two dimensions (e.g., in a drawing)

14
Perceptual DevelopmentRecognition of Objects
Depth
  • By 7 months, infants use visual cues to judge
    depth distance
  • infants with a patch over one eye will reach
    toward the larger of two identical pictures of a
    face, apparently perceiving it as closer
  • infants ability to recognize objects in two
    dimensions leads to increased interest in picture
    books and television at this age

15
Perceptual DevelopmentRecognition of Objects and
Depth
  • Haptic perception perception of the properties
    of an object using touch
  • newborns can distinguish different properties of
    objects by using their mouths
  • between 4 and 6 months, infants explore objects
    actively, combining hand, mouth, and vision
  • after 6 months, infants develop specialized hand
    movements to detect information about specific
    object properties such as size, texture, and
    shape

16
Perceptual DevelopmentOther Perceptual
Developments
  • By 6 months, babies
  • recognize differences between simple melodies
  • can use cross-modal perception to infer
    information about object properties
  • infants who are familiarized with an object only
    by touch can recognize the object by sight alone
  • if babies hear a sound in the dark, they will
    reach for an object in the direction of the sound

17
Perceptual DevelopmentOther Perceptual
Developments
  • In short, 6- to 9-month-olds use subtle cues to
    infer regularities in their perceptual world
  • they can learn from pictures
    in books
    and on television
  • they pick up relationships
    between
    different senses
    to pay attention
    to things that
    interest them most
  • These perceptual abilities lead to clear
    preferences (e.g., for particular pictures,
    objects, and tastes)

Picture from flickr.com/photos/offwithyourhead/80
276937/
18
Cognitive DevelopmentMemory
  • By 7 months,
  • infants can remember how to make a mobile move
    for as long as 21 days, without a reminder
  • memories are less context dependent
  • infants can remember a salient event that has
    been learned in different (but related)
    situations
  • infants can remember longer sequences of events,
    like longer melodies
  • However, memory is still tied to the situation

19
Cognitive DevelopmentInformation Processing
  • At 7 months, infants are able to group stimuli
    into higher-order conceptual categories
  • 7-month-olds (but not 5-month-olds) recognized
    the same faces shown in different positions
  • they distinguished smiling from non-smiling faces
  • they recognize a prototype from distorted
    versions
  • they distinguish horses from other four-legged
    mammals

20
Cognitive Development Information Processing
  • Infants of this age also
  • understand that moving objects should follow
    along their prior path of movement larger
    objects can support smaller objects
  • respond differentially when the same object is
    placed above or below another object, showing
    that they have a category for these spatial
    relationships
  • seem to have a concept of number
  • they dishabituated when a puppets jumps changed,
    from two to three or from three to two

21
Cognitive DevelopmentSecondary Circular Reactions
  • Sensorimotor Stage III (4 to 8 or 9 months)
    Secondary circular reactions
  • infants begin to repeat actions that, by chance,
    produce some effect on the objects and people in
    the environment
  • once the chance discovery is made, infants make
    deliberate, intentional attempts to repeat that
    action

22
Cognitive DevelopmentSecondary Circular Reactions
  • Infants also vary the actions in order to explore
    changes in the effect
  • they will drop objects off the edge of their high
    chairs
  • they shake objects in different ways to notice
    the effect or repeatedly dump things out of
    containers
  • Repeated occurrences in the environment take on
    meaning for the baby (see Observation 7.2)
  • by 7 months, Laurent knew that he would be fed
    shortly after he heard his mothers bed creak

23
Cognitive DevelopmentSecondary Circular Reactions
  • Infants are becoming more goal directed can
    perceive others intentional behavior
  • in one study, 9-month-old infants looked longer
    when a grasping hand contacted a toy than when
    the toy was touched with the back of a hand

Picture from flickr.com/photos/36908756_at_N00/30147
4833
24
Cognitive DevelopmentOut of Sight, Out of Mind?
  • Object permanence the ability to remain aware
    of an object even after it has gone out of sight
  • infants will not actively search for an object
    that has been hidden until after 9 months

Picture from http//scienceblogs.com/cognitivedai
ly/2005/08/do_babies_know_if_hidden_objec.php
25
Cognitive DevelopmentOut of Sight, Out of Mind?
  • In one study, 7- to 8-month-olds saw an object
    disappear behind one of two screens
  • a hand reached behind the screens and reappeared
    holding the object in either a possible situation
    or an impossible situation
  • Infants looked longer at the hand following the
    impossible situation compared to the possible one
  • In a similar study, infants not only looked
    longer at the impossible situation, they also
    looked more at their parents as if to share their
    puzzlement

26
Cognitive DevelopmentOut of Sight, Out of Mind?
  • Infants of this age are becoming aware of objects
    and people as whole entities
  • people, as opposed to objects, are understood by
    infants as having intentions
  • the ability to perceive anothers intentions
    corresponds with infants awareness of their own
    intentions, their ability to have an effect on
    the environment

27
Emotional DevelopmentNegative Emotions
  • Anger arises when infants cannot succeed at being
    an effective causal agent
  • accompanied by crying, but facial expression and
    underlying feeling are different from distress
  • In one study
  • 2- and 4-month-olds reacted to inoculations with
    physical distress, crying with tightly shut eyes
  • 7-month-olds responded with more angry
    expressions, crying with open, vigilant eyes

28
Emotional DevelopmentNegative Emotions
  • Anger can be adaptive and useful
  • In one study, infants were taught to pull a
    string to activate a slide projection music
  • After this, the experimenters stopped turning on
    the slide projector and music when the infant
    pulled
  • Most infants reacted with anger, but some showed
    sadness
  • the infants who expressed anger immediately
    became interested again when the contingency was
    renewed
  • those who showed sadness reacted with less
    enjoyment

29
Emotional DevelopmentNegative Emotions
  • Expressions of anger are also seen in
    7-month-olds when they are frustrated
  • e.g., when a teething biscuit is removed from
    their mouths or when their arms are restrained
  • Separation distress
  • after 6 months, infants respond to parental
    separation with some anger, especially if the
    parent happens to be a part of the infants
    activity (e.g., play) when he or she leaves

Picture from http//meidays.blogdrive.com/
30
Emotional DevelopmentNegative Emotions
  • Wariness
  • infants may become quiet and stare at a stranger
    or a strange situation, knit their brows, become
    momentarily sober, and look away
  • wariness allows the infant to observe what is
    happening is a more adaptive reaction to
    strange situations than the withdrawal of
    infantile fussing and crying

31
Emotional DevelopmentPositive Emotions
  • Positive emotions become more complex
  • Different types of smiles had different meanings
    depending on whether the infant gazed at the
    mother or not
  • Simple smiling gazing at mother during
    peekaboo
  • enjoyment of recognition or of readiness to
    engage in play
  • Simple smiles without gazing at mother after a
    tickle
  • often accompanied by gasping for air and sighing
    perhaps associated with enjoyment of relief or
    of relaxation

32
Emotional DevelopmentPositive Emotions
  • Duchenne smiles occur with gazing at mother when
    she uncovers her face during peekaboo
  • may reflect an enjoyment of agency, sensing
    oneself as an active rather than passive
    participant in the game
  • Duchenne smiles without gazing at mother occur
    most during a tickle, often as infants turn their
    bodies away as if trying to hide or protect
    themselves
  • may reflect an enjoyment of hiding or perhaps an
    enjoyment of escape

Picture from http//www.associatedcontent.com/ima
ge/123286/index.html?cat25
33
Emotional DevelopmentPositive Emotions
  • There is a growing ability to communicate with
    others about emotions
  • around 8 months, infants who smile when looking
    at an object will turn to smile at a nearby adult
  • By 6 months, babies will laugh
  • at jokes
  • at very abrupt and highly arousing stimuli
  • at things that once made them cry, such as a loud
    noise or a loss of balance
  • in one study, they cried when a stranger wore a
    mask, but laughed when their mothers did

34
Emotional DevelopmentEmotion Regulation
  • Infants are beginning to use cognition to decide
    what to feel, a process known as appraisal
  • there is a growing relationship between infant
    emotion attention to emotion-related events and
    processes

Picture from www.spicetart.com/growing_ivy/page/2
/
35
Emotional DevelopmentEmotion Regulation
  • Gender differences
  • In one study, 6-month-old boys and girls were
    observed during face-to-face play, followed by
    maternal still-face
  • Boys were more likely than girls to
  • smile vocalize as well as show anger or
    distress during the still-face
  • have a more positive interaction with the mother
    during the normal face-to-face period
  • Girls gazed more at objects showed more
    interest

36
Emotional DevelopmentRecognition of Emotional
Expressions
  • Between 6 and 9 months
  • babies seem more capable of recognizing smiles
    than other expressions
  • their ability to distinguish between other
    expressions, such as fear and anger, is
    relatively poor
  • Individual differences
  • 7-month-old infants whose mothers show a lot of
    positive emotions are more likely to respond to
    negative facial expressions, perhaps because of
    their relative novelty

37
Emotional DevelopmentRecognition of Emotional
Expressions
  • 7-month-olds ability to distinguish between
    emotions improves when
  • facial expressions are combined with voices
    expressing the same emotions
  • faces are presented dynamically
  • They also recognize whether a facial expression
    is paired with a matching vs. a mismatched
    intonation
  • e.g., when an angry expression is matched with an
    angry vs. happy tone of voice

38
Emotional DevelopmentRecognition of Emotional
Expressions
  • Infants of this age prefer to look at faces
    judged by adults to be attractive
  • apparently, attractiveness, like recognition of
    particular people, can be inferred from more
    global features of the face that do not involve
    specific expressions
  • Infants can also distinguish between the faces of
    children and adults

39
Emotional DevelopmentInfant Temperament
  • Temperament
  • a persistent pattern of emotion and emotion
    regulation in the infants relationship to people
    and things in the environment
  • Some aspects of temperament are partly inherited
  • negativity and inhibition appear early in life
    and are persistent in 5-10 of \ infants up until
    5 to 7 years
  • similar proportions of persistently inhibited
    children are found in different countries and
    even in infant monkeys

40
Emotional DevelopmentInfant Temperament
  • Infants who were the most inhibited
  • were more likely to be subdued in unfamiliar
    situations, have a dour mood, report anxiety, and
    have an overactive sympathetic nervous system
    response as teenagers
  • showed a higher activation in the amygdala (part
    of the limbic system responsive to fear) when
    viewing pictures of unfamiliar faces as adults

41
Emotional DevelopmentInfant Temperament
  • Infants and children who have difficulties with
    attention and emotion regulation (rated as highly
    reactive, emotional, inattentive, or inhibited)
    have different patterns of activity in the
    prefrontal cortex compared to well-regulated
    infants
  • For example, inhibition is related to brain wave
    and heart rate patterns as well as to stress
    responses to frustration
  • Stress responses to frustration such as heart
    rate acceleration, cortisol secretion, and
    sympathetic nervous system activation are
    present at an early age for some inhibited
    infants and may persist for periods of up to 1
    year

42
Emotional DevelopmentInfant Temperament
  • Role of parents
  • infants who are more inhibited are more likely to
    have parents who are introverted anxious
  • infant inhibition negativity are related to
    lower scores on maternal adaptation to pregnancy,
    sensitivity to the infant after birth, and
    self-esteem
  • mothers who rate infant cries as more aversive
    are more likely to rate their infants as
    difficult
  • These findings do not rule out a genetic
    explanations

43
Emotional DevelopmentInfant Temperament
  • A finding that may call the genetic explanation
    into question is that children do not necessarily
    exhibit continuity of temperament
  • inhibited children may, with sensitive child
    rearing, eventually lose their extreme
    sensitivity
  • normal children may become more inhibited in
    extremely stressful environments

44
Emotional DevelopmentInfant Temperament
  • Continuity discontinuity
  • extreme fussiness at birth predicts later
    emotionality in full-term infants, but not in
    premature infants
  • temperament most likely does not contribute to
    long-term cognitive deficit or enhancement
  • parental behavior may attenuate the long-term
    effects of early temperamental characteristic
  • inhibited children who showed more positive
    emotion were less likely to be inhibited at age 3
    than inhibited children who tended to be more
    negative

45
Emotional DevelopmentInfant Temperament
  • Parental and child factors can interact to
    influence the stability of temperament over time
  • Temperament assessed at the end of the infancy
    period, between 2 and 4 years of age, tends to
    show long-term stability
  • 2-year-olds who were rated as more difficult had
    more attention problems and aggressive behavior
    at 12 years
  • 3-year-olds who were rated low on self-control
    had more adjustment problems and interpersonal
    conflicts as adults

46
Emotional DevelopmentThe Measurement of
Temperament
  • Temperament is difficult to measure in a reliable
    and valid manner
  • some researches have conducted observations
  • usually, parents are asked to rate their childs
    temperaments
  • However, when mothers and fathers are asked to
    rate the same child, their reports agree only
    about half the time
  • there is more agreement about the difficulty of
    an infant than about any other dimension of
    temperament

47
Emotional DevelopmentThe Measurement of
Temperament
  • The correlation between parental reports and
    behavior observed in a laboratory improves if
    infants behaviors are extreme
  • Explanations of differences between parent
    reports and observed behavior include
  • infants behave differently in different
    situations
  • questions on rating scales dont reflect childs
    individuality
  • parental reports reflect parents personalities
    or psychological state
  • for instance, multiparous extroverted mothers
    were more likely to rate their infants as easy

48
Emotional DevelopmentThe Measurement of
Temperament
  • The best research strategy
  • a combination of parental reports, direct
    observations, and physiological measures (such as
    cortisol and sympathetic nervous system activity)
    made at repeated intervals in the childs life

Picture from http//ethiopia.adoptionblogs.com/we
blogs/african-american-dolls
49
Social and Language DevelopmentSocial-Object
Frames
  • As infants become increasingly interested in
    objects, between 4 and 6 months of age, earlier
    face-to-face frames give way to social-object
    play frames
  • the infants developmental task is to integrate
    interest in objects with the desire to remain
    socially and emotionally connected to the parents
  • At first, infants are primarily focused on the
    objects
  • it is up to the parents to provide frames for
    mutual communication about the objects

50
Social and Language DevelopmentSocial-Object
Frames
  • Coordination of attention to people objects is
    enhanced if parents regularly create
    object-directed frames
  • the more attentive animated parents are, the
    more likely that the infant will learn to
    co-regulate attention with others
  • infants who are more attentive to what adults do
    and say are more likely to learn language and to
    learn, by age 3 or 4 years, to share the mental
    perspectives of other people

Picture from private collection
51
Social and Language DevelopmentSocial-Object
Frames
  • At 6 months, infants appear to be in a receptive
    mode, ready to participate in the frames created
    by the parents
  • By 8 months, they are beginning to take
    initiatives in social frames
  • they start making jokes
  • they ask to be picked up by making sad facial
    expressions or raising their hands above their
    heads
  • they smile and laugh more toward familiar and
    trusted adults than toward unfamiliar ones

Picture from private collection
52
Social and Language DevelopmentBabbling
  • Babbling begins after 6 months
  • sounds as though babies are talking to themselves
    as they roll off a string of related vowel and
    consonant sounds to accompany their eating or
    playing
  • has the intonation contours (the rising and
    falling pitches) of sentences
  • the intonation contours of babbling match the
    intonation contours of the speech spoken in the
    infants home (e.g., French, Chinese, or Arabic)

53
Social and Language DevelopmentBabbling
  • In one study, some mothers were asked to respond
    contingently to infant babbling, while another
    group was asked not to respond to the babbling
  • infants whose mothers were contingent produced
    more mature babbles that had more recognizable
    syllables, strong contrasts between consonants
    and vowels, and a more fully voiced sound
  • this suggests that babbling may be speech-like
    because it occurs during parent-infant contingent
    vocal interaction.

54
Social and Language DevelopmentBabbling
  • Right-handed reaching and rhythmical banging
    increase at the same age infants begin to babble
  • the right hand is controlled by the left brain,
    known to be the primary location of speech
    processing
  • vocalizations come increasingly under the control
    of the left brain, setting the stage for linking
    vocalization and cognition, necessary for the
    development of speech
  • Babbling is more likely to be accompanied by
    (right-) hand arm movements than leg movements

55
Social and Language DevelopmentSpeech Perception
  • Before 6 months, infants can distinguish sound
    contrasts from many different languages
  • they start to lose this ability between 6-9
    months
  • In one developmental study
  • younger infants (4-6 months) could distinguish
    the syllable contrasts from all three languages
    tested (English, Salish, and Hindi)
  • older infants (10-12 months) could only
    distinguish between the contrasts of the language
    heard in the home

56
Social and Language DevelopmentSpeech Perception
  • The loss of perceptual sensitivity may be related
    to the selective processes of brain development
  • at first, synapses are overproduced
  • later some are selected and strengthened
    synapses for sounds that are not frequently heard
    disappear
  • By 9 months, American infants prefer to listen to
    words having a strong-weak stress pattern
  • 6-month-olds showed no such preference

57
Social and Language DevelopmentSpeech Perception
  • By the second half of the first year, infants
    begin to recognize produce some of the
    characteristics of language as a system of sounds
  • However, babbling infants are not trying to talk
  • they are exploring how to make familiar sounds,
    rather than as trying to communicate with sounds
  • it seems as if they first learn the music and
    then the words
  • this music is learned in the context of
    parent-infant frames, including social games

58
Social and Language DevelopmentParent-Infant
Games
  • By 8 months, new social frames emerge in the
    parent-infant relationship
  • infants take more initiative
  • As infants get older, they learn to play new
    social games, such as point and name and give
    and take at 12 months
  • games like gonna get you and horsie, in which
    the 6-month-old played a relatively passive role,
    occur only rarely at 12 months

59
Social and Language DevelopmentCultural
Differences
  • Climate is one factor that accounts for cultural
    differences in child-rearing patterns
  • in warm countries, infants tend to be carried,
    remain in close physical contact, and to be
    breast-fed longer than infants reared in cold
    climates
  • infants in cold climates are more likely to be
    separated from their mothers at an earlier age

Picture from saindonienne.wordpress.com/
60
Social and Language DevelopmentCultural
Differences
  • Cultural differences in parental beliefs about
    emotion regulation and communication about
    emotions
  • in North America Korea, parents were concerned
    about stimulation to foster development
  • Italian mothers had similar feelings about love
    and emotional closeness as North American mothers
    but were less focused on cognitive stimulation
  • Similar patterns are found in Latin-American
    cultures

61
Social and Language DevelopmentCultural
Differences
  • In a study of physical contact during play
    between Hispanic- and Anglo-American mothers and
    their 9-month-old infants
  • the overall amount of physical contact did not
    differ
  • Hispanic mothers touched, kissed, hugged, and
    held their infants physically closer than Anglo
    mothers
  • on questionnaires, the Hispanic mothers reported
    placing a higher value on touch and affection
    than Anglo mothers

Picture from www.momease.com
62
Social and Language DevelopmentCultural
Differences
  • In technical and industrial societies, including
    Japan, Korea, Europe, North America, and urban
    families everywhere
  • parents will begin to interpret the infants
    intentions
  • the next step is to help the infant carry out the
    intended act
  • parents often create new intentions that were not
    there in the first place
  • In nontechnical agricultural and hunter-gatherer
    communities, adults are more directive and
    ritualistic
  • Chomorro mothers (from the Pacific island of
    Guam), were highly directive repetitive when
    interacting with infants

63
Social and Language DevelopmentCultural
Differences
  • Each style has evolved to fit the needs of the
    particular culture problems may arise, however,
    when cultures are forced to interact
  • Hispanic Americans, as a minority culture in the
    United States, often feel self-conscious in the
    company of their Anglo-American neighbors and
    think of themselves as too affectionate with
    their infants
  • According to ecological systems theory, this is a
    conflict between the microsystem of the family
    and the macrosystem of the culture

64
Social and Language DevelopmentSelf-Awareness
  • Between 6 and 9 months, babies call attention to
    themselves in ways that did not exist previously
  • These features make up a sense of a
    differentiated ecological self
  • asking for help
  • taking initiative
  • clowning and showing off
  • demanding
  • hiding and escaping

Picture from private collection
65
Social and Language Development Self-Awareness
  • At this age
  • emotions become more complex
  • infants begin to take initiative
  • infants begin to seem to have their own
    personalities
  • But they do not have a sense of subjectivity
  • they have feelingsgetting angry or happybut
    cannot yet stand apart from those feelings
  • they do not have a sense of an I that feels,
    and, consequently, they do not have a sense that
    other people are separate subjects with their own
    feelings

66
Family and SocietyMaternal Employment
  • In 2001,
  • 64 of U.S. mothers with children under the age
    of 6 were in the work force
  • mothers also do a substantial amount of unpaid
    work (e.g., child care, household work),
    estimated to be worth about 27,000 per woman per
    year

67
Family and SocietyMaternal Employment
  • In general, infant-mother attachment is not
    seriously altered by maternal employment
  • If attachment is going to be affected, it is most
    likely to decline between employed mothers and
    their infant sons rather than their daughters
  • boys are perceived as more independent and as
    requiring less parental nurture and attention
    than girls, who are seen as more vulnerable
  • there is a correlation between a sons insecure
    attachment and a mothers perceived level of
    stress

68
Family and SocietyMaternal Employment
  • A number of studies find that the important
    variable is the mothers desire to work
  • problems with coping, dissatisfaction with life,
    depression, and loneliness are significantly
    higher in young mothers who do not work outside
    the home than in those who do
  • there are higher levels of functioning in
    families in which the mothers are employed

69
Family and SocietyMaternal Employment
  • Whether mothers work by choice or necessity, they
    typically end the day fatigued because role
    overload
  • the demands of a role are more than an individual
    can easily cope with or when the same person is
    required to perform too many roles
  • Role overload increases if the child has a
    difficult (age 1) or hostile aggressive (age 3)
    temperament
  • in this case, mothers a more likely to perceive
    themselves as less competent in both the
    parenting and work roles, and are more likely to
    feel depressed

70
Family and SocietyMaternal Employment
  • When women work, fathers can also experience a
    form of role overload
  • such fathers show more negative behavior with
    their infants during the first year
  • after the first year, they are just as sensitive
    to their infants as other husbands
  • When women remain at home, fathers can choose
    when and how to become involved with their
    infants
  • these fathers show more positive emotion toward
    their infants and are more attuned to the
    infants needs, especially if the infants are
    boys

71
Family and SocietyParental Leave Policies
  • Even when mothers are paid, they earn only 71
    cents for every dollar earned by a man in the
    same position
  • Some alternatives exist, but they are not
    widespread
  • more flexible work schedules (flextime) began to
    be instituted in Europe in the early 1960s
  • Swedish workers are entitled to maternity and
    paternity leaves
  • Swedish women earn about 90 the wages men do for
    similar jobs 86 of women with young children
    are in the workforce

72
Family and SocietyParental Leave Policies
  • In 1993, the U.S. Congress enacted the Family and
    Medical Leave Act of 1993 (FMLA, Public Law
    103-3), which established a family leave policy
  • provides unpaid leave from employment for up to
    12 weeks without loss of rank or position in the
    workplace in businesses with more than 50
    employees
  • applies to both mothers and fathers as well as to
    non-pregnancy-related illnesses
  • unfortunately, 95 of businesses are exempt from
    the FMLA because they have fewer than 50
    employees

73
Family and SocietyParental Leave Policies
  • Mothers are more likely to take a parental leave
  • on average, about 3 months
  • mothers who take shorter leaves are more likely
    to feel stress symptoms of depression, show
    negative emotions toward their infants and
    spouses, and to have less interest in their
    infants
  • The average length of leave for fathers was 6.5
    days, with 71 of fathers taking 5 or fewer days
  • fathers who took shorter leaves worked for
    employers who were did not have a positive
    reaction to the employees fatherhood, were less
    involved with their infants, and had less
    communication with their spouse about the infant

74
Family and SocietyParental Leave Policies
  • Compared to other industrialized countries, the
    United States is not a nation that fully supports
    children and families
  • With little opportunity to take time off from
    work
  • mothers cannot breast feed for as long as
    recommended by the American Academy of Pediatrics
    (for 12 months)
  • mothers may choose drug-assisted childbirth or
    C-sections, even if they would have preferred a
    natural birth, to get back to work sooner

75
Family and SocietyNurturance Toward Infants
  • Interest and ability to care for babies
  • is present in young boys and girls
  • will continue in both if fostered by the
    environment
  • In one study,
  • girls approached a baby more than did boys
  • once the children were near the baby, both boys
    and girls spoke to, reached out for, and touched
    the baby equally
  • In another study,
  • boys and girls approached babies equally
  • at 2 and 3 years, boys were more likely to
    approach male babies, girls were more likely to
    approach female babies

76
Family and Society Nurturance Toward Infants
  • Preschoolers speech to babies
  • both boy and girl preschoolers modify their
    speech to babies to make it sound more like
    motherese
  • most preschoolers rarely asked questions of the
    baby (question asking is a major form of adult
    speech to infants)
  • 25 of preschoolers used endearing terms toward
    the baby and asked soliciting questions (Are you
    hungry? Are you getting frustrated?)

77
Family and Society Nurturance Toward Infants
  • Girls aged 8 to 14 interact more with babies and
    ignore them less than boys do
  • boys and girls are equally physiologically
    aroused or unaroused by the sight of an infant
  • differences in male vs. female interest in babies
    continue through high school but seem to vanish
    for college students young adults
  • Parents vs. nonparents
  • the most responsive group is usually new mothers
  • mens child-rearing status does not affect their
    responsiveness to babies

78
Family and Society Parenthood Mothers versus
Fathers
  • Mothers tend to be more accurate in identifying
    the type of cry (pain, distress, etc.) than
    fathers
  • Both mothers and fathers can distinguish their
    own infants cry from cries of unfamiliar infants

Picture from http//www.julienna.com/pictures/Dad
20and20Baby20on20Slide.jpg
79
Family and SocietyParenthood Mothers versus
Fathers
  • Father-infant interaction
  • used to be less contingently responsive than
    mothers but no differences were found in recent
    studies
  • play and interaction are more directive show
    abrupt changes of activity
  • cultural differences in the amount of father
    involvement
  • Mother-infant interaction
  • games are quieter and depend more on the pace set
    by the infant
  • engage in more caregiving

80
Family and SocietyGrandparents
  • One study found that grandparents of infants were
    more responsive to babies than were parents of
    adolescents or of grown children who had left
    home
  • grandmothers were more responsive than
    grandfathers
  • grandfathers were more responsive than men at
    other ages
  • Grandmother-infant attachment at 1 year
  • when grandmothers spend much time with the baby,
    mothers grandmothers are nearly interchangeable
    as attachment figures
  • the more time a grandmother spends with the baby,
    the more secure the attachment relationship

81
Family and SocietyGrandparents
  • In the 1990s, mothers and grandmothers were
    generally in agreement over beliefs
  • mothers were more accepting of messiness and
    nudity indoors, more relaxed about when to begin
    toilet training, and less rigid in
    differentiating sex roles in child play
  • Some studies have shown that African-American
    grandmothers are more involved with their infant
    grandchildren than Caucasian-Americans
  • extended family is important in the reduction of
    family stress, esp. for low-income, teenage, and
    single mothers

82
Experiential ExercisesRolling Over
  • This exercise is about the connection between the
    core and its influence on an infants movements
  • Lying on your back, place an object on the floor
    directly above your head.
  • Turn your head to the right and try to look at
    your object, so that your back begins to arch.
    Relax for 30 seconds, then repeat.
  • You may notice that your back is arching so much
    so that you end up on your side. Once this
    happens, try and balance yourself like a see-saw.
  • Once you are balanced, relax your core see if
    you fall to one side or the other
  • After the roll, you may notice that one arm is
    trapped underneath you. Flex your core so as to
    create a space between your chest and the floor
    to allow you to free your arm.
  • Now look at your object. Can you reach it?

83
Experiential ExercisesBeginning to Crawl
  • Crouched on your hands and knees on the floor,
    slowly, raise one knee, hold it for a few
    seconds, and bring it back down.
  • Notice how your weight shifts when you do this
    motion.
  • Again, very slowly, raise your other knee and
    repeat the movement. Do this also with both of
    your hands.
  • Bring your knees together so that your feet are
    close enough to touch. Repeat the slow-motion
    raising and lowering movements with both of your
    legs. Notice how this feels
  • Now, spread your knees on the ground far apart.
    Repeat the same slow-motion movements. Notice how
    this feels.
  • Experiment with different spacing between your
    knees. Where is the best balance between
    instability and stability?

84
Experiential ExercisesBeginning to Crawl
  • Slowly lift your right hand and your right knee
    at the same time. How is your weight shifted and
    distributed? Do the same with your left hand and
    knee.
  • Next try lifting opposite hands and knees. Is
    this easier?
  • Rock back and forth on your hands, shifting your
    weight to and from your heels. Does this feel
    like getting ready to move?
  • Crawl a few steps forward. Notice the order of
    limbs that you move. If you lift your hands and
    knees diagonallym this is called contralateral
    crawling. If you lift your hands and knees on the
    same side, this is called homolateral crawling.
  • Try crawling a few steps backward. Is this easier
    than going forward? Babies often crawl backwards
    before they crawl forwards.
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com