Eye Physiology - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

1 / 65
About This Presentation
Title:

Eye Physiology

Description:

Because the different background intensities, the small squares do not ... For example, the American flag will not immediately appear red, white, and blue ... – PowerPoint PPT presentation

Number of Views:663
Avg rating:3.0/5.0
Slides: 66
Provided by: garyechr
Category:

less

Transcript and Presenter's Notes

Title: Eye Physiology


1
Eye Physiology
  • The following figure shows the anatomy of the
    human eye in cross section

2
Eye Physiology
  • There are two types of receptors in the retina
  • The rods are long slender receptors
  • The cones are generally shorter and thicker in
    structure
  • The rods and cones are not distributed evenly
    around the retina.
  • Rods and cones operate differently
  • Rods are more sensitive to light than cones.
  • At low levels of illumination the rods provide a
    visual response called scotopic vision
  • Cones respond to higher levels of illumination
    their response is called photopic vision

3
(No Transcript)
4
(No Transcript)
5
Eye Physiology
  • Rods are more sensitive to light than the cones.

6
Eye Physiology
  • The eye contains about 6.5 million cones and 100
    million rods distributed over the retina.
  • The density of the cones is greatest at the
    fovea, this is the region of sharpest photopic
    vision.

7
Eye Physiology
  • There are three basic types of cones in the
    retina
  • These cones have different absorption
    characteristics as a function of wavelength with
    peak absorptions in the red, green, and blue
    regions of the optical spectrum.
  • is blue, b is green, and g is red
  • There is a relatively low sensitivity to blue
    light
  • There is a lot of overlap

8
Eye Physiology
  • The optic nerve bundle contains on the order of
    800,000 nerve fibers.
  • There are over 100,000,000 receptors in the
    retina.
  • Therefore, the rods and cones must be
    interconnected to nerve fibers on a many-to-one
    basis.

9
Contrast Sensitivity
0
1
2
3
4
Circle constant
Background constant
Just noticeable difference (JND) at 2
10
Contrast Sensitivity
0
1
2
3
4
Circle constant
Background constant
Just noticeable difference (JND) at 2
11
Contrast Sensitivity
0
1
2
3
4
Backgrounddifferent thenboth halves
Backgroundsame asright half
Just noticeable difference (JND) 4 (top) and
2 (bottom)
12
Contrast Sensitivity
0
1
2
3
4
Backgrounddifferent thenboth halves
Backgroundsame asright half
Just noticeable difference (JND) 4 (top) and
2 (bottom)
13
Contrast Sensitivity
  • The response of the eye to changes in the
    intensity of illumination is nonlinear
  • Consider a patch of light of intensity idI
    surrounded by a background intensity I as shown
    in the following figure

14
Contrast Sensitivity
  • Over a wide range of intensities, it is found
    that the ratio dI/I, called the Weber fraction,
    is nearly constant at a value of about 0.02.
  • This does not hold at very low or very high
    intensities
  • Furthermore, contrast sensitivity is dependent on
    the intensity of the surround. Consider the
    second panel of the previous figure.

15
Logarithmic Response of Cones and Rods
  • The response of the cones and rods to light is
    nonlinear. In fact many image processing systems
    assume that the eye's response is logarithmic
    instead of linear with respect to intensity.
  • To test the hypothesis that the response of the
    cones and rods are logarithmic, we examine the
    following two cases
  • If the intensity response of the receptors to
    intensity is linear, then the derivative of the
    response with respect to intensity should be a
    constant. This is not the case as seen in the
    next figure.

16
(No Transcript)
17
Logarithmic Response of Cones and Rods
  • To show that the response to intensity is
    logarithmic, we take the logarithm of the
    intensity response and then take the derivative
    with respect to intensity. This derivative is
    nearly a constant proving that intensity response
    of cones and rods can be modeled as a logarithmic
    response.
  • Another way to see this is the following, note
    that the differential of the logarithm of
    intensity is d(log(I)) dI/I. Figure 2.3-1
    shows the plot of dI/I for the intensity response
    of the human visual system.
  • Since this plot is nearly constant in the middle
    frequencies, we again conclude that the intensity
    response of cones and rods can be modeled as a
    logarithmic response.

18
Simultaneous Contrast
  • The simultaneous contrast phenomenon is
    illustrated below.
  • The small squares in each image are the same
    intensity.
  • Because the different background intensities, the
    small squares do not appear equally bright.

19
Simultaneous Contrast
  • Perceiving the two squares on different
    backgrounds as different, even though they are in
    fact identical, is called the simultaneous
    contrast effect.
  • Psychophysically, we say this effect is caused by
    the difference in the backgrounds, but what is
    the physiological mechanism behind this effect?

20
Simultaneous Contrast
  • Perceiving the two squares on different
    backgrounds as different, even though they are in
    fact identical, is called the simultaneous
    contrast effect.
  • Psychophysically, we say this effect is caused by
    the difference in the backgrounds, but what is
    the physiological mechanism behind this effect?

Lateral Inhibition
21
Lateral Inhibition
  • Record signal from nerve fiber of receptor A.
  • Illumination of receptor A alone causes a large
    response.
  • Add illumination to three nearby receptors at B
    causes the response at A to decrease.
  • Increasing the illumination of B further
    decreases As response.
  • Thus, illumination of the neighboring receptors
    inhibited the firing of receptor A.
  • This inhibition is called lateral inhibition
    because it is transmitted laterally, across the
    retina, in a structure called the lateral plexus.

22
Lateral Inhibition
23
Lateral Inhibition
  • A neural signal is assumed to be generated by a
    weighted contribution of many spatially adjacent
    rods and cones.
  • Some receptors exert an inhibitory influence on
    the neural response.
  • The weighting values are, in effect, the impulse
    response of the human visual system beyond the
    retina.

24
Lateral Inhibition
25
Mach Band Effect
  • Another effect that can be explained by the
    lateral inhibition.
  • The Mach band effect is illustrated in the figure
    below.
  • The intensity is uniform over the width of each
    bar.
  • However, the visual appearance is that each strip
    is darker at its right side than its left.

26
Mach Band
  • The Mach band effect is illustrated in the figure
    below.
  • A bright bar appears at position B and a dark bar
    appears at D.

27
Modulation Transfer Function (MTF) experiment
  • An observer is shown two sine wave grating
    transparencies, a reference grating of constant
    contrast and spatial frequency, and a
    variable-contrast test grating whose spatial
    frequency is set at some value different from
    that of the reference.
  • Contrast is defined as the ratio (max-min)/(max
    min)where max and min are the maximum and
    minimum of the grating intensity, respectively.
  • The contrast of the test grating is varied until
    the brightness of the bright and dark regions of
    the two transparencies appear identical.

28
Modulation Transfer Function (MTF) experiment
  • In this manner it is possible to develop a plot
    of the MTF of the human visual system.
  • Note that the response is nearly linear for an
    exponential sine wave grating.

29
  • Note that the envelope of the visible bars
    generally follows the MTF curves of the previous
    figure.

30
Logarithmic model for monochrome vision
  • It has been postulated that the nonlinear
    response of the eye to intensity variations is
    logarithmic in nature and occurs near the
    beginning of the visual information processing
    system.
  • Below is a simple logarithmic eye model for
    monochromatic vision.

31
Monochrome Vision Model
  • The logarithmic/linear system eye model provides
    a reasonable prediction of visual response over a
    wide range of intensities.
  • However, at high spatial frequencies and at very
    low or very high intensities, observed responses
    depart from responses predicted by the model.

32
Lateral Inhibition
33
Extended model for Monochrome Vision
34
Subjective Color
  • Intermittent pulses of white light are perceived
    as colored light.
  • 1894 Benham invented the following experiment.
  • Spinning the disk CCW
  • Outer ring appears red
  • Middle ring appears green
  • Inner ring appears blue
  • CW rotation reverses thecolors of the inner and
    outer rings.
  • This effect is due to the temporal response of
    the human visual system to flashing lights.

35
Chromatic Adaption
  • The hue of a perceived color is dependent on the
    adaption of a viewer.
  • For example, the American flag will not
    immediately appear red, white, and blue if the
    viewer has been subjected to high-intensity red
    light before viewing the flag.
  • The colors of the flag will appear to shift in
    hue toward the red complement, cyan.

36
(No Transcript)
37
Color Blindness
  • Approximately 8 of males and 1 of females are
    subject to some form of color blindness.
  • Monochromats only possess rods or rods plus one
    type of cone.
  • Dichromats possess two of the three types of
    cones.
  • Both monochromats and dichromats can distinguish
    colors insofar as they have learned to associate
    particular colors with particular objects.

38
Classroom Experiments
  • Use Matlab to reproduce the Weber Fraction plot.
  • Use Matlab to reproduce the Mach band effect with
    uniform intensity bars (figure 2.3-2).
  • Use Matlab to reproduce the Mach band effect with
    the sigmoid intensity profile (figure 2.3-2).
  • Use Matlab to reproduce the simultaneous contrast
    phenomenon (figure 2.3-3).
  • Use Matlab to display a simple American flag and
    a solid red square to reproduce the chromatic
    adaption experiment from Pratt.

39
Color Vision Model
  • Trichromatic Model of human color vision
  • The eye possesses 3 types of sensors, each
    sensitive over a different wavelength band
  • Receptor spectral sensitivities s1(l), s2(l), and
    s3(l) represent the absorption pigments of the
    retina.
  • Receptors produce signalswhere C(l) is the
    spectral energy distribution of the incident
    light source.

40
Color Vision Model
  • Spectral Sensitivities si(l) in Fig 2.2-4 where
    obtained by spectral absorption measurements of
    cone pigments.
  • Direct physiological measurements are difficult
    to perform accurately.
  • Indirect estimates of cone spectral sensitivities
    have been obtained from measurements of the color
    response of color blind individuals Konig and
    Brodhun.

41
Color Vision Model
  • The 3 signals e1, e2, e3 are subject to a
    logarithmic transfer function and combined to
    produce the outputs.

42
Color Vision Model
  • Finally, the signals d1, d2, d3 pass through
    linear systems with transfer functions H1, H2,
    and H3, to produce the output signals g1, g2, g3
    that provide the basis for perception of color by
    the brain.

43
Color Vision Model
  • d2 and d3 are related to the chromaticity of a
    colored light.
  • d1 is proportional to luminance.
  • This model satisfies the basic laws of
    colorimetry.
  • If the spectral energy of a colored light changes
    by a constant multiplicative factor, the hue and
    saturation of light, as described by its
    chromaticity coordinates, remains invariant over
    a wide dynamic range, i.e., d2 and d3 do not
    change.
  • The luminance d1 increases in a logarithmic
    manner.

44
Color Vision Model
  • Just like the monochrome model, the logarithmic
    color vision model may be extend to a more
    accurate model.
  • The linear transfer functions HE1, HE2, HE3,
    account for the optical response of the eye
  • A point non-linearity is substituted for the
    logarithmic transfer function.

45
Color Vision Model
  • Sine wave response measurements for colored
    lights were performed by van der Horst, de Weert,
    and Bouman.
  • Chromatic response is shifted toward low spatial
    frequencies relative to the luminance response.

46
Gamma Correction
  • Red, green, and blue signals from video camera
    sensors typically are linearly proportional to
    the light striking each sensor.
  • However, light generated by CRT displays is
    approximately equal to the display amplitude
    drive signals raised to a power in the range of
    2.0 to 3.0.
  • Gamma correction is a compensation process that
    corrects for this nonlinear difference.
  • The camera sensor signal is passed through a
    nonlinear system with a power, typically, of
    about 0.45.

47
Gamma Correction
  • In a video system, luminance of each of the
    linear-light red, green, and blue (tristimulus)
    components is transformed to a nonlinear video
    signal by gamma correction, which is universally
    done at the camera.
  • The Rec. 709 transfer function takes linear-light
    tristimulus value (here L) to a nonlinear
    component (here E'), for example, voltage in a
    video system

48
Gamma Correction
  • The linear segment near black minimizes the
    effect of sensor noise in practical cameras and
    scanners. Here is a graph of the Rec. 709
    transfer function, for a signal range from zero
    to unity

49
Gamma Correction
  • An idealized monitor inverts the transform
  • Real monitors are not as exact as this equation
    suggests, and have no linear segment, but the
    precise definition is necessary for accurate
    intermediate processing in the linear-light
    domain.
  • In a color system, an identical transfer function
    is applied to each of the three tristimulus
    (linear-light) RGB components.

50
Gamma Correction
  • The nonlinearity of a CRT is a function of the
    electrostatics of the cathode and the grid of an
    electron gun it has nothing to do with the
    phosphor.
  • The nonlinearity is a power function f (x)
    xa, not an exponential function f (x) ex.

51
Gamma Correction
  • To reduce TV receiver cost, gamma correction is
    performed at the television camera rather than
    the receiver.
  • A linear RGB image that has been gamma corrected
    is called a gamma RGB image.
  • LCD displays are reasonably linear in the sense
    that the light generated is approximately
    proportional to the display amplitude drive
    signal.
  • LCDs usually employ circuitry to compensate for
    the gamma correction at the sensor.

52
Gamma Correction
  • In video, a 0.45-power function is applied at the
    camera
  • Synthetic computer graphics calculates the
    interaction of light and objects. These
    interactions are in the physical domain, and must
    be calculated in linear-light values. It is
    conventional in computer graphics to store
    linear-light values in the frame buffer, and
    introduce gamma correction at the lookup table at
    the output of the frame buffer.

53
Gamma Correction
  • If linear-light is represented in just eight
    bits, near black the steps between codes will be
    perceptible as banding in smoothly-shaded images.
    This is the eight-bit bottleneck in the sketch.
  • Desktop computers are optimized neither for image
    synthesis nor for video. They have programmable
    "gamma" and either poor standards or no
    standards. Consequently, image interchange among
    desktop computers is fraught with difficulty.

54
References
  • J.S. Lim, Two-Dimensional Signal and Image
    Processing, Prentice Hall, 1991.
  • W.K. Pratt, Digital Image Processing, Wiley
    Interscience, 3rd ed., 2001.
  • Charles Poynton's Frequently Asked Questions
    about Colorhttp//www.inforamp.net/poynton/Color
    FAQ.html
  • Charles Poynton's Frequently Asked Questions
    about Gamma.http//www.inforamp.net/poynton/Gamm
    aFAQ.html

55
(No Transcript)
56
Psychophysical Vision Properties
  • Let E(l) represent the spectral energy
    distribution of light emitted from some primary
    light source.
  • Let t(l) and r(l) denote the wavelength dependent
    transmissivity and reflectivity, respectively, of
    an object
  • For a transmissive object, the observed light
    spectral energy distribution is
  • C(l) t(l)E(l)
  • and for a reflective object
  • C(l) r(l)E(l).

57
Photometry
  • The Commission Internationale de lEclairage
    (C.I.E.) sets the standards for light and color.
  • SA is a tungsten filament lamp
  • SB approximates direct sunlight
  • SC approximates light from an overcast sky.
  • A hypothetical source, called illuminant E, is
    assumed to emit constant radiant energy at all
    wavelengths.

58
Color Matching Quiz
  • Is this Additive or Subtractive Color Matching?

59
Color Matching Quiz
  • Is this Additive or Subtractive Color Matching?

60
Color Matching Quiz
  • Is this Additive or Subtractive Color Matching?

61
Color Matching
  • Additive Color Matching

Subtractive Color Matching
cyan
red
magenta
green
blue
yellow
62
Subtractive Color Matching
  • The dye concentrations of the three spectral
    filters are varied until a perceptual match is
    obtained with a reference white W.
  • The dye concentrations are recorded as A1 (W), A2
    (W), A3(W).

63
Subtractive Color Matching
  • Next, the dye concentrations of the three
    spectral filters are varied until a perceptual
    match is obtained for the colored light C.
  • If a match is possible, record the intensities
    asA1 (C), A2 (C), A3(C).
  • The tristimulus values are computed as

64
Subtractive Color Matching
  • One primary P3 is superimposed with the light
    C and compared to the overlap of the other two
    primaries P1 and P2.
  • All primaries are adjusted until there is a
    match.
  • If a match is possible, the tristimulus values
    are computed as

65
Subtractive Color Matching
  • Two primaries P2 and P3 are superimposed with
    the light C and compared to the other primary
    P1.
  • All primaries are adjusted until there is a
    match.
  • If a match is possible, the tristimulus values
    are computed as
Write a Comment
User Comments (0)
About PowerShow.com