Title: Aucun titre de diapositive
1Workshop on Features, Segments, Tones Konstanz,
30 October-1 November, 2005
Symposium on Phonological Theory Representations
and Architecture CUNY, February 20-21, 2004
Feature-based Explanation in Phonological
Inventories
Features and Sound Inventories
Nick Clements Laboratoire de Phonétique et
Phonologie, Paris E-mail clements_at_idf.ext.jussieu
.fr
Nick Clements Laboratoire de Phonétique et
Phonologie, Paris clements_at_idf.ext.jussieu.fr
2Summary
Symposium on Phonological Theory Representations
and Architecture CUNY, February 20-21, 2004
Phonological inventories are structured in terms
of a number of interacting principles which
operate on distinctive features, rather than
segments or phonetic parameters. Five general
principles are discussed and exemplified with
respect to data drawn from a large sample of
segment inventories
Features and Sound Inventories
Nick Clements Laboratoire de Phonétique et
Phonologie, Paris E-mail clements_at_idf.ext.jussieu
.fr
- feature bounding
- feature economy
- marked feature avoidance
- robustness
- phonological enhancement
3INTRODUCTION
4WHY DO LANGUAGES TEND TO HAVE CERTAIN SETS OF
SPEECH SOUNDS AND NOT OTHERS?
- A common observation not just any set of
consonants and vowels can make up a sound system.
- A central finding of the earliest work in
phonology was that sound systems are structured
in terms of correlations defined in terms of
recurrent features. (see e.g. Trubetzkoy 1939,
Martinet 1955, Hockett 1955)
5- In recent years, however, the question of
inventory structure has been more vigorously
debated among phoneticians than among
phonologists. This work has tended to minimize
the role of features. Examples
- Adaptive dispersion theory (e.g. Lindblom 1986,
Lindblom Maddieson 1988) - maximal (or sufficient) dispersion
- articulatory ease
- Gestural economy (Maddieson 1995)
- economize gestures
6- Work in mainstream Optimality Theory has tended
to neglect inventory structure, since constraint
systems evaluate individual forms rather than
system-wide generalizations. - See, however, Boersma (1997), Flemming (2002) for
proposals to incorporate system-level principles
such as dispersion, symmetry and articulatory
effort into OT.
- Paul Boersma, Functional Phonology, 1988
- Edward Flemming, Auditory Representations in
Phonology, 2002
These approaches, too, have sought explanation in
phonetic, rather than phonological principles.
7ARE FEATURES NECESSARY AT ALL?
- Some phonologists have concluded that
phonological theory no longer requires a
restrictive inventory of distinctive features but
that "phonological representation can include the
entire sea of predictable or freely varying
phonetic detail"
(Kirchner, Robert. 2001. "Phonological contrast
and articulatory effort," In Linda Lombardi,
ed., Segmental Phonology in Optimality Theory, p.
112.)
8A FEATURE-BASED APPROACH
- This talk reviews a range of evidence showing
that features play a central role in the
structuring of sound systems.
- It proposes a number of general principles stated
in terms of features, and - shows that these principles make largely accurate
predictions regarding the structure of speech
sound inventories.
9FEATURE FRAMEWORK
1. A fairly conservative set of features will be
sufficient for our purposes (e.g. Halle
Clements 1983, Sagey 1986) 2. For phonetic
feature definitions, we assume the framework of
Quantal-enhancement theory as developed by
Stevens and his collaborators (e.g. Stevens
1972, 1989, 2004, Stevens Keyser 1989, 2001)
10METHOD
11DATA BASE
- Evidence is drawn primarily from the expanded
UPSID data base (Maddieson Precoda 1989).
Properties
- contains 451 phoneme inventories (representing
6-7 of the world's languages) - geographically and genetically balanced
- electronic database facilitates rapid searches
- results can be independently verified by others
12PROBLEMS WITH THE UPSID DATA BASE
- inevitable genetic skewing (e.g. Niger-Congo 55
languages, Basque 1 language) - heterogeneity of sources, disagreements in
analyses - inclusion of some allophonic details but not
others (e.g. dental vs. alveolar stops, but not
apical vs. laminal stops) - many coding errors
To a considerable exent, these problems are
alleviated by the sheer size of the sample
however, care must be taken in interpreting
results (see Basbøl 1985, Maddieson 1991, Simpson
1999, Clements 2003)
Statistical testing (chi square) is used here to
evaluate trends.
13FEATURE BOUNDING
14FEATURE BOUNDING
- Features set an upper limit on the number of
sounds and contrasts that a language may employ
in its lexicon and phonology.
1) Sounds
Given a set of n features, a language may have at
most 2n distinctive sounds. For example,
- a language using 2 features can have up to 4
sounds (22) - one using 3 features can have up to 8 sounds
(23), etc.
15EXAMPLE MAJOR PLACE CATEGORIES
- The features posterior and distributed
define four major place categories in coronal
sounds.
apico-
lamino- retroflex
postalveolar/ anterior
anterior
palatal posterior - - - - distributed -
-
16- 2) Contrasts
- Features also set limits on the number of
contrasts a language may have. - Maximum number of contrasts (S S-1) / 2,
where S number of sounds. - Example
- 4 sounds define 6 contrasts (4 3) / 2
6 - Since the two binary features posterior and
distributed define up to 4 sounds, they
predict as many as 6 contrasts, and no more.
17ALL 6 CONTRASTS PREDICTED BY FEATURE THEORY ARE
ATTESTED
- contrast example found in e.g.
- apical anterior vs. laminal anterior apical t
vs. laminal t Temne - apical anterior vs. apical posterior apical t
vs. retroflex ÿ Yanyuwa - apical anterior vs. laminal posterior apical
t vs. palatal c Arrernte - laminal anterior vs. apical posterior laminal
t vs. retroflex ÿ Arrernte - laminal anterior vs. laminal posterior laminal
t vs. palatal c Hungarian - apical posterior vs. laminal posterior
retroflex ÿ vs. palatal c Sindhi - Moreover, no other primary coronal contrasts were
discovered in either plosives or affricates in a
survey of several hundred languages (Clements
1999).
18PHONETIC CATEGORIES ARE LESS RESTRICTIVE
- Traditional phonetic theory provides 7 (or more)
different place distinctions within this region
("apico-dental", "apico-alveolar",
"lamino-dental", "lamino-alveolar",
"palato-alveolar", "retroflex", and "palatal").
It projects as many as 21 contrasts. - Max no. sounds Max no. contrasts
- Feature theory 4
6 - Traditional phonetic theory 7
21
19FEATURE ECONOMY
20- Feature Economy is the tendency to maximize
feature combinations in a given system - - Clements (2003a,b) after sources in de
Groot (1931), Martinet (1955, 1968) -
- This principle can be observed in most speech
sound inventories, regardless of their size. - ...
21A STANDARD VARIETY OF ENGLISH 24 CONSONANTS
- ph th tSh kh
- b d dZ g
- f T s S
- v D z Z
- m n N
- w l r y h
- voiced cross-classifies all obstruents
- continuant doubles the number again
- nasal creates nasal stops at three places of
articulation
22THE ECONOMY INDEX
- Feature economy can be quantified in terms of a
measure called the economy index. Given a system
using F features to characterize S sounds, we can
define its economy index E (to a first
approximation) by the expression - E S/F
- Example English has 24 consonants and requires a
minimum of 9 features to distinguish them - labial, dorsal, continuant, voiced,
glottal, strident, posterior, nasal,
lateral - The economy index of the English consonant system
is therefore 24/9, or 2.7
23Examples sounds features E English
24 9 2.7 English 1
sound 25 9 2.8 English - 1
feature 24 8 3.0
Feature Economy can now be more exactly defined
as the tendency to maximize E. This goal can
be accomplished either by - increasing the
number of sounds S, or - decreasing the number
of features F
24TESTING FEATURE ECONOMY
- A testable prediction of feature economy is
Mutual Attraction - "A given speech sound will have a higher than
expected frequency in inventories in which all
its features are distinctively present in other
sounds." - For instance, a stop with a certain laryngeal
feature L should occur more frequently in systems
having other stops with the same feature L. - Let us look at an example.
-
25COMPARISONS OF PAIRS OF STOPS SHARING MANNER
FEATURES, BUT DIFFERING IN PLACE
- All comparisons are positive at a high level of
significance (plt.0001). That is, languages
having one member of each pair tend strongly to
have the other.
26CROSS-CATEGORY ATTRACTION
Feature economy also applies across manner
categories.
- For example, it predicts that a language having
the sounds - P T K, B D G, and F S X
- will tend to also have the sounds V Z Ä, thereby
maximizing the use of voiced and
continuant. - Result (Clements 2003)
- voiced labial fricatives V are much more frequent
than expected in languages also having P, B, and
F - analogous results hold for Z and Ä
- these trends are significant at the .0001 level
27MARKED FEATURE AVOIDANCE
28- Markedness is understood here as the systematic
avoidance of certain widely disfavored feature
values -- the marked values (Trubetzkoy 1939,
Jakobson 1941, Greenberg 1968, Chomsky Halle
1968, Kean 1980, Calabrese 1994, 2005, Rice
2002). - Markedness counteracts the free operation of
Feature Economy
- in the absence of markedness, sound systems
making use of n features would be expected to
display the theoretical maximum of 2n sounds - no languages come close to approaching this
maximum instead, segments characterized by
marked feature values tend to be avoided
29Recall the English consonant system
ph th tSh kh b d dZ g f T s S v
D z Z m n N w l r y h
Absent feature combinations correspond largely to
cross-linguistically disfavored consonant types
30At the same time, Feature Economy counteracts
Markedness
Example voiced fricatives
- voiced fricatives involve the marked feature
values voiced and continuant. - voiced fricatives are absent in roughly half the
world's languages. - however, due to the effect of feature economy, if
a language has one voiced fricative, it is twice
as likely to have another. -
/ . . .
31VOICED FRICATIVES IN UPSID
- labial V (overall 32.6 )
- in languages having no other voiced fricative
13.5 - in languages having another voiced fricative
60.3 - coronal Z (overall 38.6 )
- in languages having no other voiced fricative
16.3 - in languages having another voiced fricative
73.7 - dorsal Ä (overall 15.5 )
- in languages having no other voiced fricative
3.3 - in languages having another voiced fricative
29.2 -
32HOW DO WE KNOW WHICH VALUE OF A FEATURE IS MARKED?
- Phonetic approaches
- Phonetic theory involves an extremely rich
set of interacting principles that
frequently lead to conflicting
expectations. - example which value of nasal is
marked? - Statistical approaches
- The likelier (more frequent, more
predictable) value of a feature is
its unmarked specification (Kean 1980, Hume 2004) - A statistical approach has the advantage of
relating markedness to observable frequency
distributions that can be readily extracted by
language learners (Pierrehumbert 2003)
33- As pointed out by Greenberg (1966) and others,
markedness is reflected in frequency differences
at many levels. For example, sounds bearing
marked feature values tend to be less frequent - in the lexicon
- in running texts
- in early stages of language acquisition
- in adult sound inventories
34A PROPOSED CRITERION MARKEDNESS AS NONUBIQUITY
- A feature value is marked if it is absent in some
language in classes of sounds it which it is
potentially distinctive otherwise it is
unmarked. Examples
35THE MARKED SUBSET PRINCIPLE (MSP)
"Within any class of sounds in which a given
feature F is potentially distinctive, sounds
bearing marked values of F are less frequent
than sounds bearing unmarked values of F" In
other words, languages tend to avoid marked
feature values, regardless of the class of sounds
they occur in. The prediction is that this
principle will hold except where overridden by a
competing principle.
36SOME PREDICTIONS OF THE MARKED SUBSET PRINCIPLE
( lt is to be read are less frequent than)
a. in the class of vowels, nasal vowels lt oral
vowels (marked feature
nasal) b. in the class of consonants,
sonorants lt obstruents (marked
feature sonorant) c. in the class of
obstruents, fricatives lt stops
(marked feature continuant) Do these
predictions hold? Consider again English. s
37Prediction (a) nasal vowels lt oral vowels
true (English has no nasal vowels)
Prediction (b) sonorants lt obstruents
true (see below)
ph th tSh kh b d dZ g f T s S v
D z Z m n N w l r y h
Prediction (c) fricatives lt stops
false ! why ?
38A COMMON TYPE OF EXCEPTION TO THE MARKED SUBSET
PRINCIPLE
The number of marked sounds is often equal to the
number of unmarked sounds. Examples
English fricatives stops Ikwere nasal
vowels oral vowels i iâ u u) I Iâ U U) e
e) o o) E E)